Italian Version of the Defense Style Questionnaire Pietro San Martini, Paolo Roma, Sara Sarti, Vittorio Lingiardi, and Michael Bond The Defense Style Questionnaire (DSQ) assesses defensive behavior by empirically evaluating conscious derivatives of defense and coping mechanisms in everyday life. It was developed on the assumption that defenses can be ordered along a maturity-immaturity continuum and tend to group into clusters, or defensive styles. The original factor analytical study, by Bond et al. (1983) identified four styles, called maladaptive, image-distorting, self-sacrifice, and adaptive styles. Successive studies only partially confirmed this factor structure. We present the factor structure and the main psychometric features of the Italian version of the questionnaire. The DSQ was translated into Italian by the back-translation method and administered to a sample of 294 men (mean age, 33.33 years) and 333 women (mean age, 32.38 years). An exploratory factor analysis identified three factors largely corresponding to Bond’s maladaptive, image- distorting, and adaptive defensive styles and to analogous factors identified by other authors. Accordingly, three defense style scales were constructed, containing respectively 37, 17, and 12 items. These scales showed intercorrelations compatible with the hierarchical model of defensive functioning at the base of the questionnaire, acceptable, though ameliorable, test-retest reliabilities (r’s ⴝ .79, .63, and .81, respectively) and, with the exception of the Adaptive Style scale, sufficient internal consistencies (alphas: .85, .72, .57). However, only the Maladaptive Style scale, probably due to its greater length, showed values of reliability and internal consistency high enough to warrant clinical use in its present form. Further investigation is required to find new items that may improve the reliability of the Image-Distorting and the Adaptive Style scales. © 2004 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. T of subjectivity, which necessarily distinguish clinical opinion, in 1983 Bond et al12 proposed the first version of the Defense Style Questionnaire (DSQ), a self-administered questionnaire developed to assess defensive styles. The use of a self-report questionnaire to assess defensive behaviors seems to be in contrast to the classical psychoanalytic concept of defenses as unconscious processes and, therefore, inaccessible to the ego. In other words, if defenses are considered to be processes out of consciousness, how can we measure them through a self-report instrument? Integrating concepts like defense and coping mechanisms partially helped to overcome this contradiction. The authors of the DSQ coped with this question in two steps. On one hand, they point out that temporary failures of a defense can make individuals aware of the (more or less automatic) ways they use to defend themselves from unacceptable or distressing feelings and impulses. On the other hand, the conscious- HE NOTION of ego defenses is unanimously recognized as one of the most important psychoanalytic contributions.1 Sigmund Freud, the inventor of this construct,2 stated that defense mechanisms are “the cornerstone on which the whole structure of psychoanalysis rests”3 (p. 16). In the mid-1990s, the idea of defensive mechanisms was included in the DSM-IV4 among the axes suggested for further studies.5-7 Defenses are defined in the DSM-IV as “automatic psychological processes that protect the individual against anxiety and from the awareness of internal and external stressors”4 (p. 715). In this way, clinicians are encouraged to consider this aspect of personality as an important part of diagnosis and treatment. Despite the importance and the usefulness of this construct, an important question remains unanswered: Can we “measure” defensive mechanisms in a reliable way? Among the instruments available today for evaluating defense mechanisms, one of the most interesting is the Defense Mechanisms Rating Scales (DMRS).8,9 The DSM-IV Defensive Functioning Scale (DFS) widely refers to the hierarchal structure of the DMRS. Even though the DMRS has a good inter-rater reliability, in both the original10 and the Italian versions,11 it is based on the assessment of video-/audiotaped interviews and is still difficult to use on a large level. Moreover, its administration requires an accurate personal training course. With the open intention of creating an instrument that is easy to handle and without elements From the Department of Dynamic and Clinical Psychology, University of Rome “La Sapienza,”Rome, Italy; and McGill University and Sir Mortimer B. Davis Jewish General Hospital, Montreal, Canada. Address reprint requests to Pietro San Martini, Dipartimento di Psicologia Dinamica e Clinica Via dei Marsi, 78, 00185 Roma, Italy. E-mail: [email protected] © 2004 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. 0010-440X/04/4506-0013$30.00/0 doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2004.07.012 Comprehensive Psychiatry, Vol. 45, No. 6 (November/December), 2004: pp 483-494 483 484 ness of their own defensive operations can be accentuated from other people comments and behaviors. Moreover, the habitual usage of some defenses leaves a “print” in some of the individual’s usual attitudes. A sentence like “I’m able to keep a problem out of my mind until I have time to deal with it” could point to a habitual attitude, a “forma mentis” that individuals use as a defense mechanism (suppression) to cope with a stressor or a conflict. In this way, the DSQ is created for stimulating individuals to express their own ways of coping with conflicts and stressors, and it is based on the clinical assumption that people can describe their past behavior reliably, taking for granted that only a deep clinical investigation can identify unconscious processes at the very moment they happen. Thus, we should remember that the DSQ evaluates “conscious derivatives”12 of defensive mechanisms. Moreover, the aim of the DSQ is not just to measure defense mechanisms separately, but groups of defenses called defensive styles, as, according to Bond,13 measuring individual defenses is not reliable. The authors created this questionnaire starting from the hierarchical model of Vaillant,14 which distinguished four defensive levels based on a immaturity–maturity or pathology–mental health continuum and, by using the empirical method of factor analysis, they distinguished four groups or defensive styles, which, even though they did not correspond with the theoretical levels of Vaillant as a whole, nevertheless could be arranged according to its continuum. These styles are as follows: (1) maladaptive style—composed of acting-out, regression, passive-aggressive behavior, withdrawal, projection, and inhibition; individuals who endorse this style show a basic incapability of coping with internal or external stressors by using actions or behaviors for their own advantage. (2) Image-distorting style—composed of omnipotence-devaluation, splitting, primitive idealization; individuals who endorse this pattern basically split the selfimage and the image of others in extreme classes avoiding a total view of reality that could prove to be too much stressful. (3) Self-sacrifice style— composed of defenses like pseudo-altruism and reaction formation; people who use it tend to deny their own needs and focus on being kind to others. (4) Adaptive style—composed of sublimation, humor, and suppression; individuals who use it tend SAN MARTINI ET AL to be able to put distressing feelings out of their minds by focusing on either the humor in situations, or the “silver linings,” or by channeling their feelings into constructive, creative activities. In 1986 Bond improved the original version of the DSQ by increasing the consistency of its scales. The final version is composed of 88 items linked to 25 defense mechanisms and of a “lie” scale. Each defense mechanism is measured by a minimum of one item (pseudo-altruism, sublimation, projective identification, and fantasy) to a maximum of nine items (projection). Several studies performed in different countries examined the dimensional structure of the questionnaire, with results only partially corresponding to those originally found by Bond et al.12 The Australian research of Andrews et al.,15 based on 271 normal individuals and 142 psychiatric patients, showed a three-factor solution. Two of the factors were similar to the extreme factors, in terms of Vaillant’s continuum, found by Bond, that is, an immature factor and a mature factor. The third factor included defenses belonging both to Bond’s image-distorting and self-sacrifice styles. Reister et al.16 analyzed the responses obtained from a sample of 301 normal subjects with the German version of the questionnaire and found four defense factors that could be ordered on the maturity-immaturity continuum and that, as they state (a table of the factor loadings is not presented), had the same clinical meaning as the factors described by Bond. Sammallhati and Alberg,17 in a Finnish sample of 73 patients and 353 normal subjects, extracted four factors. The first factor, labeled “immature style“ included all the defenses of Bond’s maladaptive style with the addition of fantasy, somatization, consumption, and help-rejecting complaining. The second factor was a ”mature style“ factor, including task orientation, humor, and sublimation. The third factor, labeled ”borderline style,“ included omnipotence-devaluation, isolation, denial, splitting, and projective identification. The fourth factor, labeled “neurotic style” included reaction formation, undoing, pseudo-altruism, and primitive idealization. Despite differences in the labels, on the whole, the nature of the factors extracted shows strong similarities with Bond’s styles. More recently, Rutherford et al.18 studied two different versions of the questionnaire in a sample of 215 patients undergoing methadone therapy. ITALIAN VERSION OF THE DSQ They used rather strict criteria to decide on the number of factors, among them the criterion that each retained factor should contain at least five defenses with loadings ⱖ.30, and found two factors for both versions. The first factor included 16 defenses normally considered immature or neurotic, and the second depicted more mature defenses (task orientation, anticipation, reaction formation, suppression, and sublimation). As the latter factor showed insufficient internal consistencies (Cronbach’s alphas: .64 and .66), the authors concluded that the instrument could be properly used to measure only one dimension of immature defenses. Bonsack et al.,19 using a French version of the questionnaire, found, both in their control group of 82 normal subjects and in their 140 psychiatric outpatients, four factors, ranging from immature to mature defense styles. Although they state that, except for minor differences, “. . .the psychometric features of the French version are similar to the original scale. . .” a thorough analysis of their factor structure shows remarkable differences in the content of the factors compared to Bond’s original scales. The above studies support the notion that defenses can be assembled in clusters on a continuum of maturity–immaturity and/or pathology–mental health; however, they show considerable differences concerning the number of defensive clusters to be distinguished and where to set their boundaries. All of them agree in showing a cluster of maladaptive defenses, whereas the defenses placed on the more mature levels on Vaillant’s continuum present a rather inconstant segmentation. The DSQ, in its original version or in a shortened 40-item version,20 has been used in the following different clinical areas: eating disorders,21-27 anxiety disorders,28-31 depressive disorders,32-34 personality disorders,17,35-41 alexithymia,42-44 various problems in adolescence.45-52 Furthermore, the DSQ has been studied as a possible process and outcome predictor of short-term psychotherapy.53,54 Within this context our aims are: (1) to confirm, through factor analysis, that the defensive mechanisms, as measured by the Italian version of the DSQ, gather in clusters that can be ordered on the maturity–immaturity and/or mental health–mental pathology continuum proposed by Vaillant; (2) to evaluate the internal and longitudinal reliability of the defensive style scales constructed on the basis of these clusters; and (3) 485 to analyze the association between the defensive styles and the personality dimensions assessed by the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire-Revised (EPQ-R).55 METHODS Subjects Subjects were 628 volunteers between 17 and 66 years of age: 294 males (mean age, 33.33; SD ⫽ 12.04) and 333 females (mean age, 32.38; SD ⫽ 11.03). They were university students (39%), civil servants and teachers (38%), and professionals (14%), living in Rome (approximately 90%) and in other cities and small communities of the same area. For the most part they were informally recruited during 2 successive years on the university campus, in the libraries and faculties of the University of Rome “La Sapienza,” and in the administration offices of some banks, town halls, public schools, and other public departments in the region of Rome. On the whole, they may be considered an opportunistic sample of the population living in the urban and suburban area of Rome, with a prevalence of middle-class people with high school and university education. All participants provided written informed consent after complete description of the study. Instruments All respondents completed a booklet containing the items of the DSQ and the items of a short form of the EPQ-R. The items of the latter instrument were interspersed randomly among the items of the DSQ. The Defense Style Questionnaire. The 88-item version of the DSQ56 was translated into Italian using the back-translation method.57 The Italian and the original versions of the questionnaire and the distribution of the items among the 25 defense mechanisms are displayed in Appendices I and II. A 9-point Likert-type scale indicating degree of agreement or disagreement with the item content follows the items. The Eysenck Personality Questionnaire-Revised. The short form of the EPQ-R used in this study was developed by two of the authors of the present report in two steps. On the basis of data from a previous factor analytical study on the Italian version of the full form of the questionnaire,58 for each of the four scales, extraversion, neuroticism, psychoticism, and lie, the items that correlated most strongly with the respective factors and were factorially pure were selected. This 40-item form of the EPQ-R was successively administered to a new sample of 428 subjects, for the most part university students. A principal component analysis followed by Varimax rotation of the first four components showed close correspondence between factors of the scales; internal reliabilities were acceptable and scale intercorrelations were similar to those reported for the British short form.55 For the purpose of the present study, the EPQ-R questions have been rephrased into assertions followed by 9-point Likertlike response scales identical to those of the DSQ. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION Descriptive Statistics and Internal Consistencies Descriptive statistics and internal consistencies of the sets of items representing the 25 486 SAN MARTINI ET AL Table 1. Descriptive Statistics and Internal Consistencies of the Defense Mechanisms Men Defense Mechanisms Acting-out Affiliation Undoing Anticipation Passive-aggressive Consumption Denial Fantasy Reaction formation Primitive idealization Projective identification Inhibition Isolation Help-rejecting complaining Omnipotence Task-orientation Projection Pseudo-altruism Regression Suppression Withdrawal Splitting Somatization Sublimation Humor Mean 3.52 2.79 2.67 4.86 2.74 1.94 1.80 4.52 2.80 3.14 .98 2.96 3.10 2.22 2.71 4.87 1.62 5.69 2.30 3.94 4.56 3.45 1.97 2.05 4.69 defenses and coping mechanisms are displayed in Table 1 for males and females separately. As expected, given the low number of items in each set, internal consistencies are generally moderate, particularly so for the sets representing suppression and denial. An exception is the decidedly low internal consistency of consumption (.16), a set of three items. This low value may be at least partly due to one of the items of this set, item 79 (“I take drugs, alcohol or medicine when I am tense”) that, in our nonpsychiatric sample, had a strongly positively skewed distribution with low mean and standard deviation (M ⫽ 0.5; SD ⫽ 1.67). Not surprisingly, this item had near zero (r ⬍ .16) correlations with the remaining two items. We could not find in the literature measures of internal consistency of the individual defense sets for a direct comparison. We note, however, that with the exception of consumption, our internal consistencies are in line with the data of Bond et al.,12 as the items of these a priori–constructed sets were retained by Bond et al. if their item-total correlations were significant at the .001 level, that Women SD 1.77 2.05 1.67 2.10 1.47 1.67 1.29 2.87 1.60 2.29 1.86 1.72 1.65 1.97 1.61 2.37 1.06 2.10 2.02 2.08 2.05 2.09 2.09 2.65 1.84 Mean 4.06 3.48 2.60 4.97 2.81 2.56 1.43 4.78 2.93 3.62 1.51 3.56 2.47 2.28 2.27 5.18 1.79 6.22 3.31 3.58 5.47 3.40 2.97 2.60 4.57 SD Cronbach’s Alpha 1.66 2.24 1.80 2.13 1.45 1.70 1.32 2.83 1.60 2.58 2.45 1.84 1.59 1.96 1.58 2.12 1.19 1.91 2.12 2.18 1.93 1.98 2.28 2.98 1.93 .69 .32 .36 .34 .32 .16 .27 .57 .43 .67 .41 .66 .71 .59 .68 .43 .26 .61 .48 .46 .54 is, considering the number of their subjects, if their item-total correlations were above .22. Factor Analysis As a preliminary step, a maximum liklihood confirmatory factor analysis was performed, to assess the goodness of fit of the four dimensional model originally proposed by Bond et al.12 on the basis of their exploratory factor analyses. Only the 14 defense sets representing the maladaptive, image-distorting, self-sacrifice, and adaptive styles in the original study were included in the analysis, which allowed factor components to be correlated. For the analysis, the SEPATH module of the package STATISTICA (StatSoft, Tulsa, OK) was used. Several measures were employed to estimate the degree to which the model fit the data, among them the chi-square statistic, the goodness-of-fit index (GFI), the adjusted goodness-of-fit index (AGFI), and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). The 2 value was 482.91 (df ⫽ 71; P ⫽ .000), the GFI was .890, the AGFI was . 838, and the point estimate of the RMSEA was .102 with a 90% confidence interval between .094 and .111. ITALIAN VERSION OF THE DSQ Fig. 1 487 Scree plot of the eigenvalues of DSQ scores. Overall, these indices showed an insufficient fit and suggested that an amelioration of the model was appropriate. For this purpose, we decided to adopt an exploratory rather than confirmatory perspective in the subsequent analysis of the dimensional structure of the questionnaire. A principal component analysis was performed on the correlation matrix of all the 25 item sets. The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Index of sampling adequacy was .83, indicating that the variables shared sufficient common variance to justify factor analysis. According to the scree-plot (Fig 1), it was clearly appropriate to extract four factors, explaining 42.1% of the total variance. These four factors were rotated by the Quartimax method (Table 2), the same rotation method used by Bond et al.12and by the majority of the subsequent studies. A Varimax rotation was also performed, with substantially identical results and will not be further considered. The rotated factor matrix is displayed in Table 2. In the following description, an item will be said to load saliently on a factor if it has its maximum loading on that factor, provided that loading exceeds .39. The first factor, explaining 19.81% of the variance, is loaded saliently by 11 defenses, all of them usually considered immature or neurotic. In terms of defense content, it is similar to the first factor obtained by Bond et al.12 with the previous form of the questionnaire, sharing with it the following defenses: regression, projection, somatization, acting-out, passive-aggressive Table 2. Rotated Factor Matrix Factors Defense Mechanisms Regression Acting-out Projection Somatization Passive-aggressive Withdrawal Fantasy Consumption Help-rejecting complaining Projective identification Undoing Denial Omnipotence Isolation of affect Splitting Anticipation Task-orientation Pseudo-altruism Primitive idealisation Sublimation Suppression Affiliation Inhibition Humor Reaction formation Explained variance: 42.07% NOTE. Values below .25 are omitted. 1 .760 .690 .637 .618 .559 .541 .530 .515 .472 .461 .421 .334 ⫺.253 2 3 ⫺.316 .432 ⫺.256 .257 .257 .250 .438 .295 .385 .667 .650 .647 .466 .308 .321 ⫺.433 .288 .604 .598 .461 .415 .412 .401 .282 .466 19.81% 4 8.33% .405 .348 7.23% ⫺257 .621 ⫺.499 .493 6.74% 488 behavior, withdrawal, consumption, and projective identification. The factor is loaded saliently also by fantasy, which was not included in the 1983 version of the questionnaire. This defense was found to belong to the immature factor also by Andrews et al.15 and by Sammallahti et al.17 Inhibition, which in Bond et al.12 belonged to the immature factor, in our sample loads maximally on the fourth factor, but has a high secondary loading on the first factor as well. Help-rejecting complaining and undoing also have salient loadings on the first factor; however, the former with a secondary loading on the second factor and the latter with similar secondary loadings on the second and third factors. On the whole, the content of this factor corresponds quite closely to the concept of maladaptive style as defined by Bond et al.12 The second factor, explaining 8.33% of the variance, is loaded saliently by denial, omnipotencedevaluation, isolation, and splitting, the same defenses constituting the borderline style factor extracted by Sammallahti et al.17 This factor shares omnipotence-devaluation and splitting with the image-distorting style found by Bond et al.12 Although this defensive style is often seen in borderline patients, we preferred to label it imagedistorting, rather than borderline, to use a term that is less tied to a specific psychiatric diagnosis. The third factor, explaining 7.23% of the variance, is loaded saliently by anticipation, task-orientation, pseudo-altruism, primitive idealization, sublimation, and suppression. Humor, which loads maximally (negatively) on the forth factor, has a high (positive) secondary saturation on this factor as well. It is clearly a cluster of adaptive defenses, although the presence of items of pseudo-altruism and primitive idealization requires some comment. The (single) item representing pseudo-altruism contains two different statements: “I get satisfaction from helping others” and “If this were taken away from me, I would get depressed.” We think that our respondents could have focused their attention predominantly on the first statement that was in a way directly related to their actual situation, as they were in fact just helping someone else doing a research. In other words, this item might have been perceived by our subjects, rather than a maladaptive defensive behavior, as a normal expression of solidarity (see the DFS of the DSM-IV for differential definitions of pseudo-altruism and altruism).4 As for primitive idealization (which in the studies conducted by Bond et al. in 198312 and by Sammallahti et al. in 199517 weighted significantly on more than one fac- SAN MARTINI ET AL Table 3. Correlations Between DSQ Factor Scores and EPQ-R Scale Scores Factor Factor Factor Factor 1 2 3 4 Psychoticism Neuroticism Extraversion .03 .33 ⫺.14 .02 .78 .12 ⫺.04 .07 ⫺.21 .18 .36 ⫺.49 NOTE. All values ⬎.07 are significant at .05 level tor), we should consider that, depending on the situation, it may function as a healthy process, inspiring progressive behavior. In the Italian, mainly Catholic, cultural context, item 51 (“I always feel that someone I know is like a guardian angel”) may be perceived as reflecting ways of thinking and expressing oneself that are more common and considered as more “normal” than in the United States or even in Canada. A similar interpretation might be proposed for the other item of this defense (no. 58:“There is someone I know who can do anything and who is absolutely fair and just”). We decided to label this factor adaptive style. The fourth factor, explaining 6.7% of the variance, is saliently loaded by the defenses of inhibition, reactive formation, and, negatively, humor, all with secondary high loadings on the first and third factors. Even though the factor has no clear markers, it may tentatively be interpreted as an “introverted” or “over-inhibited” style. Correlations With the EPQ-R Scales DSQ factor scores were calculated and correlated with the four EPQ-R scale scores (Table 3). There was a high correlation between the immature factor and neuroticism (r ⫽ .78), a moderate correlation between the mature factor and extraversion (r ⫽ .36), and a moderate positive correlation between the image-distorting factor and psychoticism (r ⫽ .33). The fourth factor showed a moderate negative correlation (r ⫽ –.49) with extraversion, not unexpected considering that humor was one of its three components. On the whole, the pattern of correlations is compatible with the interpretation of the factors given above. Psychometric Features of the Factorial Scales Leaving aside the fourth factor that was composed only of (three) factorially complex defenses, we constructed three factorial scales, composed of all defenses loading saliently on the respective factors. Descriptive statistics, internal consistencies (Cronbach’s alphas) gender differences, and ITALIAN VERSION OF THE DSQ 489 Table 4. Descriptive Statistics, Internal Consistencies, and Significance of Gender Differences for the Three Style Scales Maladaptive style Image-distorting style Adaptive style Men (n ⫽ 289) Women (n ⫽ 327) No. of Items Mean SD Mean SD Cronbach’s alpha P for the t Test 37 17 12 2.54 2.70 4.25 1.05 1.11 1.05 2.94 2.31 4.40 1.03 1.10 1.81 .85 .72 .57 .0000 .0000 .0040 test-retest correlations of these scales, evaluated on the item level, are shown in Table 4. Retest was measured after 6 months on a subsample of 47 subjects (21 men and 26 women, aged 20 to 58 years; mean, 34 years; SD ⫽ 11.3). Internal consistencies were satisfactory for the Maladaptive Style scale, which comprised 37 items, just sufficient for the Image-Distorting Style scale, which comprised 17 items, and decidedly low for the Adaptive Style scale, which comprised 12 items. The differences among these values were likely to be at least partly due to the different scale lengths. In fact, when evaluated on the basis of 37 items with the Spearman-Brown prophetic formula, the internal consistencies of the last two scales became satisfactory (r ⫽ .81 for both scales) and similar to that of the Maladaptive Style scale. Test-retest correlations were adequate for the Maladaptive Style scale (r ⫽ .79; P ⬍ .001) and for the Adaptive Style scale (r ⫽ .81; P ⬍ .001), and a bit low for the Image-Distorting Style scale (r ⫽ .63; P ⬍ .001). Compared to males, females scored significantly higher than males in the maladaptive style and significantly lower on the image-distorting and on the mature styles. Correlations Between the Factor Scales Premise. According to the model, on the basis of the questionnaire the defenses can be ordered along a maturity–immaturity continuum. As a consequence, defense styles are not conceived as completely independent, that is, defense styles that are close on the continuum are expected to be positively correlated and defense styles that are far on the continuum are expected to be less (positively) correlated, not correlated, or negatively correlated. From this point of view, the orthogonal rotation of the factors, which was performed following the method of Bond et al.,12 should be interpreted as a way of forcing the defenses into clusters that, if meaningful, may be used as measuring scales. There was a moderate positive correlation between the maladaptive and the image-distorting styles (r ⫽ .44; P ⬍ .001), a weak positive correlation between the image-distorting and the adaptive styles (r ⫽ .21; P ⬍ .001), and a weak positive correlation between the adaptive and maladaptive styles (r ⫽ .18; P ⬍ .001). With the exception of the last correlation, which is discrepant with the negative correlation between the maladaptive and mature styles reported by Bond et al.,12 on the whole this pattern of scale intercorrelations is in agreement with Bond’s data and compatible with his model. CONCLUSIONS Three factors, identified as maladaptive, imagedistorting, and adaptive styles, emerged from our data. These factors, despite some difference in the labels used to describe them, were very similar to three of the factors reported both by Bond et al.12 and by Sammallahti et al.17 and were correlated in a predictable way with the EPQ-R scales. The scales constructed on the basis of the factor analysis showed intercorrelations compatible with the hierarchical model of defensive functioning at the base of the questionnaire, acceptable test-retest reliabilities, and internal consistencies with the exception of the consistency of the Adaptive Style scale and the test-retest reliability of the ImageDistorting Style scale, that were lower than desirable. Only the Maladaptive Style scale, which, compared to the other two scales, had a much larger number of items, showed values of reliability and internal consistency high enough to warrant use in its present form. Before investigating other aspects of the validity of the questionnaire (e.g., its concurrent and discriminant validity) it therefore seems useful to try to improve these psychometric faults of the shorter scales by finding new suitable items and bring their lengths to the level of the Maladaptive Style scale. 490 SAN MARTINI ET AL APPENDIX I. The DSQ Items Item No. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 Original Version Italian Version I get satisfaction from helping others and if this were taken away from me I would get depressed. People often call me a sulker. I’m able to keep a problem out of my mind until I have time to deal with it. I’m always treated unfairly. I work out my anxiety through doing something constructive and creative like painting or woodwork. Once in a while I put off until tomorrow what I ought to do today. I keep getting into the same type of frustrating situations and I don’t know why. I’m able to laugh at myself pretty easily. I act like a child when I’m frustrated. I’m very shy about standing up for my rights with people. I am superior to most people I know. Provo soddisfazione nell’aiutare gli altri e mi deprimerei se mi si impedisse di farlo. Le persone mi dicono spesso che sono scontroso. Quando ho un problema sono capace di non pensarci fino a quando non ho tempo per affrontarlo. Mi trattano sempre ingiustamente Gestisco l’ansia facendo cose costruttive e creative come, ad esempio, dipingere o fare lavori con il legno. People tend to mistreat me. If someone mugged me and stole my money, I’d rather he’d be helped than punished. Once in a while I think of things too bad to talk about. Once in a while I laugh at a dirty joke. People say I’m like an ostrich with my head buried in the sand. In other words, I tend to ignore unpleasant facts as if they didn’t exist. I stop myself from going all out in a competition. I often feel superior to people I’m with. Someone is robbing me emotionally of all I’ve got. I get angry sometimes. I often am driven to act impulsively. I’d rather starve than be forced to eat. I ignore danger as if I were Superman. I pride myself on my ability to cut people down to size. People tell me I have a persecution complex. Sometimes when I am not feeling well I am cross. I often act impulsively when something is bothering me. I get physically ill when things aren’t going well for me. I’m a very inhibited person. I’m a real put-down artist. I do not always tell the truth. I withdraw from people when I feel hurt. I often push myself so far that other people have to set limits for me. My friends see me as a clown. I withdraw when I’m angry. I tend to be on my guard with people who turn out to be more friendly than I would have suspected. Ogni tanto rimando a domani ciò che dovrei fare oggi. Mi metto continuamente nello stesso tipo di situazioni frustranti e non so perchè. Mi è abbastanza facile ironizzare su me stesso. Quando sono frustrato mi comporto come un bambino. Sono molto timido net difendere i miei diritti con le persone. Sono superiore alla maggior parte delle persone che. conosco. La gente tende a maltrattarmi. Se qualcuno mi scippasse rubandomi del denaro, preferirei che fosse aiutato piuttosto che punito. Talvolta mi capita di pensare a cose troppo brutte per parlarne. Talvolta mi capita di ridere ad una barzelletta sporca. Mi dicono che metto la testa sotto la sabbia come gli struzzi. In altre parole che, di fronte a fatti spiacevoli, tendo a fare come se non esistessero. Nelle competizioni, evito di espormi completamente Spesso mi sento superiore alle persone che sono con me. Qualcuno mi sta derubando di tutte le mie emozioni. Qualche volta mi arrabbio. Spesso mi sento spinto ad agire impulsivamente. Preferirei morire di fame piuttosto che essere forzato a mangiare. Ignore il pericolo come se fossi Superman. Sono orgoglioso della mia capacità di fare “abbassare la cresta” alle persone. La gente mi dice che ho il complesso di persecuzione. Talvolta, quando non mi sento in salute, divento di cattivo umore Spesso, quando c’è qualcosa che mi disturba, reagisco impulsivamente. Mi ammalo fisicamente quando le cose non mi vanno benc Sono una persona molto inibita. Sono un artista nello “smontare” gli altri. Non sempre dico la verità. Mi ritraggo dalle persone quando mi sento offeso. Spesso mi spingo cosı̀ in là che gli altri sono costretti a frenarmi. Gli amici mi considerano un “giocherellone”. Quando sono arrabbiato tendo a ritrarmi in me stesso. Tendo a stare in guardia con le persone che si rivelano più amichevoli di quanto mi aspetti. ITALIAN VERSION OF THE DSQ 491 APPENDIX I. The DSQ Items (Cont’d) 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 I’ve got special talents that allow me to go through life with no problems. Sometimes at elections I vote for men about whom I know very little. I’m often late for appointments. I work more things out in my daydreams than in my real life. I’m very shy about approaching people. I fear nothing. Sometimes I think I’m an angel and other times I think I’m a devil. I would rather win than lose in a game. I get very sarcastic when I’m angry. I get openly aggressive when I feel hurt. I believe in turning the other cheek when someone hurts me. I do not read every editorial in the newspaper every day. I withdraw when I’m sad. I’m shy about sex. I always feel that someone I know is like a guardian angel. My philosophy is “Hear no evil, do no evil, see no evil.” As far as I’m concerned, people are either good or bad. If my boss bugged me, I might make a mistake in my work or work more slowly so as to get back at him. Everyone is against me. I try to be nice to people I don’t like. I would be very nervous if an airplane in which I was flying lost an engine. There is someone I know who can do anything and who is absolutely fair and just. I can keep the lid on my feelings if it would interfere with what I’m doing if I were to let them out. Some people are plotting to kill me. I’m usually able to see the funny side of an otherwise painful predicament. I get a headache when I have to do something I don’t like. I often find myself being very nice to people who by all rights I should be angry at. There’s no such thing as “finding a little good in everyone.” If you’re bad, you’re all bad. We should never get angry at people we don’t like. I am sure I get a raw deal from life. I fall apart under stress. When I know that I will have to face a difficult situation like an exam or a job interview. I try to image what it will be like and plan ways to cope with it. Doctors never really understand what is wrong with me. Ho doti particolari che mi permettono di affrontare la vita senza problemi. Qualche volta alle elezioni voto per uomini di cui so molto poco. Sono spesso in ritardo agli appuntamenti. Elaboro molte più cose nelle mie fantasticherie piuttosto che nella vita reale. Sono molto timido nell’avvicinare le persone. Non ho paura di niente. Qualche volta mi sento angelo e qualche volta un diavolo. Nei giochi preferisco vincere piuttosto che perdere. Quando sono arrabbiato divento molto sarcastico. Quando mi sento ferito divento apertamente aggressivo. Quando qualcuno mi offende ritengo giusto porgere l’altra guancia. Non sempre leggo tutti gli articoli di fondo del giornale Quando sono triste tendo a ritirarmi in me stesso. Sono timido riguardo al sesso. Ho sempre la sensazione che qualcuno che conosco sia come un angelo custode. La mia filosofia è “Non vedo, non sento, non parlo.” Per quanto mi riguarda, le persone sono o buone o cattive. Se il mio capo mi stesse troppo addosso, potrei fare deliberatamente un errore nel lavoro oppure potrei lavorare più lentamente per fargliela pagare. Mi sono tutti contro. Cerco di essere gentile con le persone che non mi piacciono. Mi innervosirei molto se l’aeroplano sul quale stessi viaggiando perdesse un motore. Conosco una persona che è in grado di fare qualsiasi cosa ed è estremamente leale e giusta. Sono in grado di tenere a freno i miei sentimenti se esprimerli interferisse con ciò che sto facendo. Alcune persone stanno congiurando per uccidermi. Solitamente sono in grado di vedere il lato comico di situazioni altrimenti penose. Mi viene mal di testa quando debbo fare qualcosa che non mi piace. Spesso mi ritrovo ad essere molto gentile con persone nel confronti delle quali avrei ogni ragione di essere arrabbiato. Non è vero che si possa “trovare un po’ di buono in ognuno”: chi è cattivo è cattivo. Non dovremmo mai arrabbiarci con le persone che non ci piacciono. Sono convinto di ricevere solo fregature. Lo stress mi manda in pezzi. Quando so di dover affrontare una situazione difficile come un esame o un colloquio di lavoro, cerco di immaginare come sarà e pianifico vari modi per affrontarla. I medici non capiscono mai che cosa ho veramente. 492 SAN MARTINI ET AL APPENDIX I. The DSQ Items (Cont’d) 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88 When someone close to me dies, I don’t feel upset. After I fight for my rights, I tend to apologize for my assertiveness. Most of what happens to me is not my responsibility. When I’m depressed or anxious, eating makes me feel better. Hard work makes me feel better. My doctors are not able to help me really get over my problems. I’m often told that I don’t show my feelings. I believe that people usually see more meaning in films, plays, or books than is actually there. I have habits or rituals which I feel compelled to do or else something terrible will happen. I take drugs, medicine or alcohol when I’m tense. When I feel bad, I try to be with someone. If I can predict that I’m going to be sad ahead of time, I can cope better. No matter how much I complain, I never get a satisfactory response. Often I find that I don’t feel anything when the situation would seem to warrant strong emotions. Sticking to the task at hand keeps me from feeling depressed or anxious. I smoke when I’m nervous. If I were in a crisis, I would seek out another person who had the same problem. I cannot be blamed for what I do wrong. If I have an aggressive thought, I feel the need to do something to compensate for it. La morte di qualcuno a me vicino non mi sconvolge. Dopo essermi impegnato per far valere i miei diritti, tendo a scusarmi per la mia assertività. Non mi sento responsabile della maggior parte delle cose che mi accadono. Quando sono depresso o ansioso, mangiare mi fa sentire meglio. Lavorare sodo mi fa sentire meglio. I dottori non sono in grado di aiutarmi veramente a risolvere i miei problemi. Mi dicono spesso che non mostro i miei sentimenti. Credo che di solito le persone trovino nei film, nelle opere teatrali o nei libri più senso di quanto realmente ce ne sia. Ho delle abitudini o rituali che sento di dover rispettare, altrimenti potrebbe accadere qualcosa di terribile. Quando sono teso assumo droghe, farmaci o alcol. Quando mi sento male cerco di stare con qualcuno. Se posso prevedere in anticipo che una situazione mi renderà triste, riesco ad affrontarla meglio. Per quanto io possa lamentarmi, non ottengo mai una risposta soddisfacente. Spesso mi trovo a non provare nulla quando la situazione sembrerebbe giustificare forti emozioni. Allontano la depressione o l’ansia impegnandomi a fondo in ciò che faccio. Fumo quando sono nervoso. Se fossi in crisi, cercherei qualcuno che avesse il mio stesso problema. Non è colpa mia se faccio qualcosa di sbagliato. Se ho un pensiero aggressivo, sento il bisogno di fare qualcosa per compensare. ITALIAN VERSION OF THE DSQ 493 APPENDIX II. The DSQ Defenses and the Items That Represent Them Defense Mechanisms Item No. Total Items Acting-out Affiliation Undoing Anticipation Passive aggressive Consumption Denial Fantasy Reaction formation Primitive idealization Projective identification Inhibition Isolation Help-rejecting complaining Omnipotence Task-orientation Projection Pseudo-altruism Regression Suppression Withdrawal Splitting Somatization Sublimation Humor Le scale 7,21,27,33,46 80,86 71,78,88 68,81 2,22,39,45,54 73,79,85 16,42,52,57 40 13,47,56,63,65 51,58 19, 10,17,29,41,50 70,76,77,83 69,75,82 11,18,23,24,30,37 74,84 4,12,25,36,55,60,66,72,87 1 9,67 3,59 32,35,49 43,53,64 28,62 5 8,34,61 6,14,15,20,26,31,38,44,48,5 7 5 2 3 2 5 3 4 1 5 2 1 5 4 3 6 2 9 1 2 2 3 3 2 1 3 10 REFERENCES 1. Buckley P. Ego Defenses. A psychoanalytic perspective. In: Conte HR, Plutchik R (eds). Ego Defenses: Theory and Measurement., et al. New York, NY: Department of Psychiatry of Albert Einstein College of Medicine of Yeshiva University, 1995:39-51. 2. Freud S. Le neuropsicosi da difesa (1894). Trans. Ital. in: Sigmund Freud, Opere, vol. 1. Torino, Italy: Boringhieri, 1967. 3. Freud S. On the history of the psychoanalitic movement (1914). In: Strachey J (ed and Trans). The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud. Vol. 14. London, UK: Hogarth Press, 1957:3-66. 4. American Psychiatric Association Committee on Nomenclature and Statistics. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Ed. 4. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press, 1994. 5. Skodol AE, Perry JC. Should an axis for defense mechanism be included in DSM-IV? Compr Psychiatry 1993;34:108-199. 6. Soldz S, Vaillant GE. A 50-year longitudinal study of defense use among inner city men: a validation of the DSM-IV Defense Axis. J Nerv Ment Dis 1998;186:104-111. 7. Perry JC, Hoglend P, Shear K, Vaillant GE, Horowitz M, Kardos ME, et al. Field trial of a diagnostic axis for defense mechanisms for DSM-IV. J Personal Disord 1998;12:56-68. 8. Perry JC. Defense Mechanism Rating Scales (DMRS). Ed. 5. Cambridge, MA: J.C. Perry, 1990. 9. Lingiardi V, Madeddu M. I Meccanismi di Difesa. Nuova edizione (contiene la versione italiana della Defense Mecha- nism Rating Scale, DMRS). Milano, Italy: Raffaello Cortina Editore, 2002. 10. Perry JC, Cooper S. An empirical study of defense mechanisms: I. Clinical interview and life vignettes ratings. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1989;46444-452. 11. Lingiardi V, Lonati C, Fossati A, Vanzulli L, Maffei C. Defense mechanisms and personality disorders. J Nerv Ment Dis 1999;187:224-228. 12. Bond MP, Gardner ST, Christian J, Sigal C. Empirical study of self-rated defense styles. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1983; 46:455-460. 13. Bond MP. The development and properties of the Defense Style Questionnaire. In: Conte HR, Plutchik R, et al (eds). Ego Defenses: Theory and Measurement. New York, NY: Department of Psychiatry of Albert Einstein College of Medicine of Yeshiva University, 1995:202-220. 14. Vaillant GE. Adaptation to Life. Boston, MA: Little Brown, 1977. 15. Andrews G, Pollock C, Stewart G. The determination of defense by questionnaire. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1989;46:455-460. 16. Reister G, Fellhauer RF, Franz M, Wirth T, Schellberg D, Schepank H, et al. Psychometric measurements of ego defense mechanisms: Correlation between questionnaire and expert rating. Psychother Psychosom Med Psychol 1993;43:15-20. 17. Sammallahti P, Aalberg V. Defense style in personality disorders—an empirical study. J Nerv Ment Dis 1995;183:516-521. 18. Rutherford MJ, Mcdermott P, Cacciola JS, Alterman AI, 494 Mulvaney F. A psychometric evaluation of the defense style questionnaire in methadone patients. J Personal Disord 1998;12:119-125. 19. Bonsack C, Despland JN, Spagnoli J. The French version of the defense stile questionnaire. Psychother Psychosomat 1998;67:24-30. 20. Andrews G, Sing M, Bond M. The Defense Style Questionnaire. J Nerv Ment Dis 1993;181:246-256. 21. Steiger H, Van Der Feen J, Goldstein C, Leichner P. Defense style and parental bonding in eating-disordered women. Int J Eat Disord 1989;8:131-140. 22. Steiger H, Goldstein C, Mongrain M, Van Der Feen J. Description of eating disordered, psychiatric, and normal women along cognitive and psychodynamic dimensions. Int J Eat Disord 1990;9:129-140. 23. Steiner H. Defense style in eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord 1990;9:141-151. 24. Sullivan PF, Bulik CM, Hall S, Weltzin TE, Kaye WH. Defense style in women with eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord 1994;16:251-256. 25. Sullivan PF, Bulik CM, Carter FA, Gendall KA, Joyce PR. The significance of a prior history of anorexia in bulimia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord 1996;20:253-261. 26. Tordjman S, Zittoun C, Ferrari P, Flament M, Jeammet P. A comparative study of defense styles of bulimic, anorexic and normal females. Isr J Psychiatry Relat Sci 1997;34:222-227. 27. Sarlio Lahteenkorva S, Rissanen A. Weight loss maintenance: determinants of long term success. Eat Weight Disord 1998;3:131-135. 28. Pollock C, Andrews G. Defense styles associated with anxiety disorders. Am J Psychiatry 1989;146:1500-1502. 29. Andrews G. Anxiety, personality, and anxiety disorders. Int Rev Psychiatry 1991;3:293-302. 30. Muris P, Merckelbach H. Defense style, trait anxiety, worry, and bodily symptoms. Pers Individ Differ 1994;16:349-351. 31. Wastell CA. Defensive focus and the Defense Style Questionnaire. J Nerv Ment Dis 1999;187:217-223. 32. Kneepkens RG, Oakley LD. Rapid improvement in the defense style of depressed women and men. J Nerv Ment Dis 1996;184:358-361. 33. Lyoo IK, Gunderson JG, Phillips KA. Personality dimensions associated with depressive personality disorder. J Personal Disord 1998;12:46-55. 34. Mullen LS, Blanco C, Vaughan SC, Vaughah R, Roose SP. Defense mechanisms and personality in depression. Depression Anxiety 1999;10:168-174. 35. Bond M. Are “borderline defenses” specific for borderline personality disorders? J Personal Disord 1990;4:251-256. 36. Johnson JG, Bornstein RF, Krukonis AB. Defense styles as predictors of personality disorder symptomatology. J Personal Disord 1992;6:408-416. 37. Bond M, Paris J, Zweig-Frank H. Defense style and borderline personality disorders. J Personal Disord 1994;8:28-31. 38. Sammallahti P, Aalberg V, Pentinsaari JP. Does defense style vary with severity of mental disorder? An empirical assessment. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1994;90:290-294. 39. Paris J, Zweig-Frank H, Bond M, Guzder J. Defense styles, hostility, and psychological risk factors in male patient with personality disorders. J Nerv Ment Dis 1996;184:153-158. 40. Mulder RT, Joyce PR, Sullivan PF, Bulik CM, Carter FA. SAN MARTINI ET AL The relationship among three models of personality psychopathology: DSM-III-R personality disorder, TCI scores and DSQ defenses. Psychol Med 1999;29:943-951. 41. Sinha BK, Watson DC. Predicting personality disorder traits with the Defense Style Questionnaire in a normal sample. J Personal Disord 1999;13:281-286. 42. Wise TN, Mann LS, Epstein S. Ego defensive styles and alexithymia: a discriminant validation study. Psychother Psychosom 1991;56:141-145. 43. Parker JDA, Taylor GJ, Bagby RM. Alexithymia: relationship with ego defense and coping styles. Compr Psychiatry 1998;39:91-98;. 44. Kooiman CG, Spinhoven P, Trijsburg RW, Rooijmans HGM. Perceived parental attitude, alexithymia and defense style in psychiatric outpatients. Psychother Psychosom 1998; 67:81-87. 45. Muris P, Merckelbach H, Bogels S. Coping, defense, and fear in college students. Pers Individ Differ 1995;18:301-304. 46. Steiner H, Feldman SS. Two approaches to the measurement of adaptive style: Comparison of normal, psychosomatically ill, and delinquent adolescents. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1995;34:180-190. 47. Erickson SJ, Feldman SS, Steiner H. Defense mechanism and adjustment in normal adolescents. Am J Psychiatry 1996;153:826-828. 48. Feldman SS, Araujo KB, Steiner H. Defense mechanisms in adolescents as function of age, sex, and mental health status. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1996;35:1344-1354. 49. LI N, Zhang H, Zhao H, Tu X. Field test of self-rated defense styles in 4,309 college students. Chin Ment Health J 1996;10:100-102. 50. Chan DW. Defense styles and psychological symptoms among Chinese adolescents in Hong Kong. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 1997;32:269-276. 51. Tuulio HA, Poikolainen K, Aalto ST, Lonnquist J. Psychological defense style in late adolescence and young adulthhood: a follow up study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1997;36:1148-1153. 52. Evans DW, Seaman JL. Developmental aspects of psychological defenses: their relation to self-complexity, self-perception, and symptomatology in adolescents. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 2000;30:237-254. 53. Piper EW, De Carufel FL, Szkrumelak N. Patient predictors of process and outcome in short-term psychotherapy. J Nerv Ment Dis 1985;173:726-733. 54. Muris P, Merckelbach H. Defense style and behaviour therapy outcome in a specific phobia. Psychol Med 1996;26: 635-639. 55. Eysenck SBG, Eysenck HJ, Barrett P. A revised version of the psychoticism scale. Pers Individ Differ 1985;6:21-29. 56. Bond MP. Defense Style Questionnaire. In: Vaillant GE (ed). Empirical Studies of Ego Mechanisms of Defense. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press, 1986. 57. Simonsen E, Mortensen E. Difficulties in translation of personality scales. J Personal Disord 1990;4:290-296. 58. San Martini P, Mazzotti E, Setaro S. Factor structure and psychometric features of the Italian version of for the EPQ-R. Pers Individ Differ 1996;21:877-882.