LIFE SPAN AND DISABILITY Journal promoted by the Department of Psychology Institute for Research on Mental Retardation and Brain Ageing “Oasi Maria SS.” - Troina CITTÀ APERTA © copyright 2009 Città Aperta Edizioni s.r.l. 94018 Troina (En) - via Conte Ruggero, 73 Tel. 0935 653530 - Fax 0935 650234 Cover by Rinaldo Cutini Printed: June 2009 by FOTOGRAF s.n.c. 90144 Palermo - Viale delle Alpi, 59 Tel. 091 228862 - Fax 091 6850243 Summary Obsessive-compulsive phenomena and symptoms in Asperger’s disorder and High-functioning Autism: An evaluative literature review Christian Fischer-Terworth, & Paul Probst 7 Cognitive profiles of genetic syndromes with Intellectual Disability Santo Di Nuovo, & Serafino Buono 31 Genetic syndromes causing Mental Retardation. Deficit and surplus in school performances and social adjustment compared with cognitive abilities Renzo Vianello, & Silvia Lanfranchi 43 Neurodevelopmental risk in preterm born babies Filippo Dipasquale, & Paola Magnano 55 Verbal communication in right hemisphere brain damage Valeria Abusamra, Hélène Côté, Yves Joanette, & Aldo Ferreres 69 International Literature Review on WHODAS II (World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule II) Stefano Federici, Fabio Meloni, & Alessandra Lo Presti 85 Life Span and Disability / XII, 1 (2009), 5-27 Obsessive-compulsive phenomena and symptoms in Asperger’s disorder and High-functioning Autism: An evaluative literature review Christian Fischer-Terworth 1 & Paul Probst 2 Abstract Although obsessional, ritualistic and stereotyped behaviors are a core feature of autistic disorders, substantial data related to those phenomena are lacking. Ritualistic and stereotyped behaviours can be found in almost all autistic patients. Additionally, cognitive able individuals with Asperger’s disorder (AD) and High-Functioning Autism (HFA: defined by the presence of IQ- levels > 70, Howlin, 2004, p. 6) mostly develop circumscribed, often called obsessional interests and preoccupations. Results from recent research indicate that autistic individuals frequently suffer from obsessions and compulsions according to DSM-IV criteria of Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder (OCD), being associated with marked distress and interference with daily life. OCD and autism share several similarities regarding symptom profiles and comorbidity. Etiologic overlap between the disorders becomes especially evident when focussing cognitive, neurobiological and genetic aspects. Autism-related obsessive-compulsive phenomena (AOCP) have generally to be differentiated from OCD-symptoms, although there is no sharp borderline. Keywords: Autistic disorders, Obsessive-compulsive phenomena, Asperger’s disorder, High Functioning Autism * Received: 10 March 2009, Revised: 22 May 2009, Accepted: 22 May 2009. 1 2 Department of Psychology, University of Hamburg. Department of Psychology, University of Hamburg, e-mail: [email protected] 7 Life Span and Disability Fischer-Terworth C. / Probst P. 1. Introduction Obsessional, ritualistic, stereotyped and repetitive behaviours are the third core feature of autistic disorders, co-occurring with communicational difficulties and problems in social interaction (DSM-IV: APA, 2000). Autism-related obsessive-compulsive phenomena (AOCP) are part of the clinical picture in all disorders of the autistic spectrum, including early childhood autism (Kanner, 1943; Probst & Hillig, 2005), high-functioning Autism (Howlin, 2004) and Asperger’s disorder (Asperger, 1944; 1944/1991). AOCP include all kinds of obsessional, compulsive, ritualistic, stereotyped and repetitive behaviours. According to the formal diagnostic criteria, social problems and the obsessive compulsive phenomena of Asperger’s disorder (AD) do not differ from those found in Early Childhood Autism (ECA) and High-Functioning-Autism (HFA). The occurrence of social deficits and obsessive compulsive phenomena in AD shows that typical autistic features also occur in individuals generally displaying higher cognitive abilities and better language skills than autistic individuals (Baron- Cohen, 2004; Wing, 2005). Recent research results (Howlin, 2004) don’t provide a basis for a separate classification of AD and HFA which also applies to obsessive compulsive phenomena (Cuccaro, Nations, Brinkley, Abramson, Wright , Hall et al., 2007). According to these findings only the development of better cognitive skills and the degree of linguistic-communicative impairment can serve as a relevant marker for differentiating AD/HFA from ECA (Baron-Cohen, 2004). Obsessions and compulsions in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorders (OCD) and Autism-related obsessive-compulsive phenomena (AOCP) are often regarded as an overlapping class of behaviors, sharing a similar genetic, biobehavioral and neurobiological basis. Case-reports of OCDsymptoms in adolescents often contain descriptions of ritualized behaviours also being common in autism (Winter & Schreibman, 2002). On the other hand AOCP often have different functions for the individual (Kennedy, Meyer, Knowles, & Shukla, 2000) than the obsessions and compulsions of typical OCD (Hand, 1992). Although cognitive deficits and language problems generally make it difficult to specify if autistic individuals display typical obsessions and compulsions (Baron-Cohen, 1989), recent studies show that many of them, including those with AD and HFA, have typical obsessions and compulsions associated with marked distress (McDougle, Kresch, Goodman, Naylor, Volkmar, Cohen, & Price, 1995; Russell, Mataix-Cols, Anson, & Murphy, 2005; Zandt, Prior, & Kyrios, 2006; Levallois, Béraud, & Jalenques, 2007). Furthermore there is some evidence for the existence of a distinct OCD subcategory being typical for autistic disorders (McDougle et al., 1995). Interestingly, recent research results show that there is a subgroup of OCD patients with autistic features (Bejerot, 2007; Ivarsson & Melin, 2008) which 8 Obsessive-compulsive phenomena and symptoms in Asperger’s disorder and High-functioning Autism may present with autism- related social and communication- related difficulties (Cullen, Samuels, Grados, Landa, Bienvenu, Liang et al., 2008). Bejerot and Mörtberg (2009) showed and an increasing risk of being bullied at school for children and adolescents with OCD associated with autistic features like low social competence. Symptoms of obsessive-compulsive disorders are time-consuming obsessions and compulsions which cause marked distress and significant interference with daily life. Obsessions are recurrent thoughts, impulses or images intruding repetitively into consciousness, often related to the fear of a threatening event for which the patient feels responsible (APA, 2000). Common obsessions are associated with fear of contamination, death or illness, other thoughts or impulses have an aggressive, sexual or blasphemous content. Compulsions are irresistible rituals the individual has to carry out over and over to reduce the anxiety or discomfort generated by the obsessions. Frequent compulsions are ritualized washing, checking (e.g. stoves or light switches), repeating (repeating words or sentences, rereading or rewriting sentences), ordering, counting, questioning and hoarding (Rasmussen & Eisen, 1992). A diagnosis of autism with comorbid OCD requires specific cognitive-behavioural (Lehmkuhl, Storch, Bodfish, & Geffken, 2008) and pharmacological interventions (Levallois, Beraud, & Jalenques, 2007) effectively targeting obsessions and compulsions, the core symptoms of OCD. These special OCD interventions should be an integral part of a treatment plan including interventions for the autistic disorder and comorbid conditions. 2. Method To review the research literature about AOCP and OCD-symptoms in autism, the online data bases “Medline”, “Psyndex” and “PsycInfo” were screened for articles from 1977 to 2009. Relevant information was also found in relevant psychiatric, psychological and educational handbooks and single publications about Autism, Asperger’s disorder and Obsessivecompulsive disorders. When screening the online data bases, the search terms “autism” and “Asperger” were combined either with “OCD”, “compulsions”, “obsessions”, “obsessive compulsive”, “rituals”, “stereotypies” and “repetitive”. In the online research the relevant articles containing descriptions, analyses and classification of obsessive-compulsive symptoms in autistic disorders were found. The selected literature consists of reviews and studies about phenomenological, genetic and etiologic overlap areas of OCD and autism, analyses of stereotyped behaviour and therapy studies and three studies especially focusing on obsessions and compulsions in autistic disorders. 9 Life Span and Disability Fischer-Terworth C. / Probst P. 3 Autism and Obsessive-compulsive Disorder: phenomenological and etiological overlap 3.1 Symptoms Less excessive variants of repetitive behaviours in OCD and autism can also be found in typically developing children (Militerni, Bravaccio, Falco, Fico, & Palermo, 2002; Greaves, Prince, Evans, & Charman, 2006). Ritualized behaviours like bedtime rituals, insistence on sameness or ritualized play mostly peak at the age between 2 and 4 and decrease with growing age (Evans, Leckman, Carter, Reznick , Henshaw, King, & Pauls, 1997). Motor stereotypies like nail biting or rocking often occur in periods of concentration, excitement or boredom (Tröster, 1994). In typical development ritualized behaviors tend to change over time which is the case in OCD and autism as well (Winter & Schreibman, 2002). Rapoport (1989) asks if the ritualized play of a four- year- old with e.g. a string could be his individual variant of a washing or checking compulsion (Winter & Schreibman, 2002). People with OCD and autistic individuals display compulsive behaviour, obsessive insistence on sameness, repetitive movements, a strong need for symmetry and certain principles of ordering (Winter & Schreibmann, 2002; Zandt et al., 2006). Ego-dystonic OCD-symptoms have generally to be separated from autism-related enjoyable “obsessional” interests (Russell et al. 2005; South, Ozonoff, & McMahon, 2005), although the distinction remains unclear (Cath, Ran, Smit, van Balkom, & Comijs, 2008). 3.2 Patterns of Comorbidity OCD and autistic disorders share a similar pattern of comorbid conditions like anxiety disorders, affective disorders, tic disorders (Rasmussen & Eisen, 1992; Howlin, 2004) or personality disorders (Bejerot, Nylander, & Lindström, 2001). Anxiety in autism (Gillott, Furniss,& Walter, 2001) is often triggered by typical autism–related problems like sensory overload or the interruption of rituals (Samet, 2006). Sensory overload can be viewed as a typical autism- related trigger for anxiety, whereas the function of rituals to reduce anxiety is similar but not identical in OCD (DSM-IV:APA, 2000). Concomitant specific phobias (Rasmussen & Eisen, 1992; Leyfert et al., 2006) social phobia (Cath et al., 2008) and separation anxiety (Bhardwaj, Agarval & Sitholey, 2005; Nestadt et al., 2003) are common in OCD and ASD. In contrast to earlier findings, comorbidity with schizophrenia and other psychotic conditions is rare in Autism (Howlin, 2004) and OCD (Frommhold, 2006). Interestingly, however, there is overlap between both AD/HFA and OCD with schizotypical symptoms and personality features (Bejerot, 2007). The personality style of many AD/HFA individuals has much in common with schizotypical personality features (reviewed by Andresen & Maß, 2001), especially regarding bizarre fantasies and interests, eccentric behaviour, metaphoric and stereotypal use of language, social and 10 Obsessive-compulsive phenomena and symptoms in Asperger’s disorder and High-functioning Autism communicational deficits (Esterberg, Trotman, Brasfield, Compton, & Walker, 2008). Also certain subcategories of OCD are more strongly related to schizotypical symptoms than to other anxiety disorders (Sobin, Blundell, Weiller, Gavigan, Haiman, & Karayiorgou, 2000). Like in tic-related OCD, a broad spectrum of clinical manifestations of comorbid tic disorders can also be found in autism (Ringman & Jankovic, 2000). Another condition frequently co-occurring with both OCD (Masi, Millepiedi, Mucci, Bertini, Pfanner, & Arcangeli, 2006) and autism (Leyfert, Folstein, Bacalman, Davis , Dinh, Morgan et al., 2006) is Attention deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), especially being prevalent in children and adolescents. Eating disorders like anorexia nervosa are a frequent comorbid condition of OCD (Kaye, Bulik, Thornton, Barbarich, & Masters, 2004) also being associated with autistic disorders, especially with AD/HFA (Jörgensen, 1994; Fisman, Steele, Short, Byrne, & Lavallee, 1996). 3.3 Etiological overlap OCD and autism both have a neurobiological basis (Winter & Schreibman, 2002). Neuroimaging studies show voluminetric and structural abnormities in the cerebellum, the frontal lobe, the hippocampus, the amygdala and the corpus callosum of autistic individuals (Herbert, Ziegler, Deutsch, O’Brien, Lange, Bakardjiev et al., 2003; Baron-Cohen, 2004; Boucher, Cowell, Howard, Broks, Farrant, Roberts, & Mayes, 2005; Boger-Meggido, Shaw, Friedman, Sparks, Artru, Giedd et al., 2006). Reports of a sudden onset of OCD symptoms following brain injury and infections show that OCD has a neurological basis (Steketee & Pigott, 2006, p. 53). Especially the effectiveness of serotonergic antidepressants and cognitive-behavioural treatment being visible in PET (Schwartz, Martin, & Baxter, 1992), SPECT and MRI scans strongly contributed to the conception of neurobiological disease models (Steketee & Pigott, 2006). Neurobiological findings in OCD and autism show similarities which might be relevant regarding obsessive compulsive phenomena (Winter & Schreibman, 2002). Patterns of familiar interaction, educational styles or other external stressors can contribute to exarcerbations, improvements or changes in both AOCP (Howlin, 2004) and OCD-symptoms (Probst, Asam, & Otto, 1979).A solid social background and the involvement in productive activities diminishes the dependence on stereotyped and ritualized behaviors in OCD (Schwartz & Beyette, 1997) and autistic disorders as well (Howlin, 2004, p. 137). 3.3.1 Cognitive aspects Similar deficits in information processing might underlie OCD and autism (Winter & Schreibman, 2002). According to several authors local information processing is preferred to global processing in autism and OCD. Additionally, impairment in the executive functions has been noted in 11 Life Span and Disability Fischer-Terworth C. / Probst P. OCD and different disorders of the autistic spectrum (Frith, 1989, 1992; Russell et al., 2005). The preference of local information processing causes the individual to focus attention on single elements of the environment which can lead to stereotypy, rituals and narrow interests (Frith, 1989, 1992). In OCD individuals are often internally forced to gain control over a small part of their environment with the attention focussed on dysfunctional thoughts and urges (Schwartz et al., 1992). An internal signal automatically terminating repetitive behaviour from a central processing unit seems to be lacking in OCD (Hoffmann & Hofmann, 2004) and ASD (Frith, 1989, 1992). Delorme, Gousse, Roy, Trandafir, Mathieu, Mouren-Simeoni et al. (2006) discuss a possible common endophenotype related to executive dysfunctions commonly associated with OCD and autism which may even be related to repetitive behavior (Zandt, Prior, & Kyrios, 2009). According to the cognitive-behavioural model of OCD (Salkovskis, Forrester, & Richards, 1997) primary neutral thoughts get filled with feelings of anxiety, disgust and/or guilt which increases their intrusiveness. Compulsions are carried out to reduce these emotions for a short period of time which reinforces the intrusiveness of the thought. As Russell et al. (2005) state, cognitive styles being typical for autistic individuals can also contribute to a specific way of giving value to those thoughts. Deficits in autonomous habituation processes, possibly being associated with amygdala dysfunction (Amaral & Corbett, 2002; Steketee & Pigott, 2006), could serve as an explanation for the obsessive resistance against change in autism and OCD. In autistic persons minimal environmental changes can trigger strong anxiety and panic attacks (Samet, 2006), whereas OCD-patients often panic when their rituals are not carried out “appropriately” (Rasmussen & Eisen, 1992). The persistent engagement in rigid obsessive-compulsive behaviours might serve as a protection against new stimuli being experienced as threatening. 3.3.2 Neuroanatomical and neurobiochemical overlap Several authors state that dysfunctions in cortico-striatal-thalamic pathways are involved in the pathogenesis of autism (Damasio & Maurer, 1978) and are a crucial element of the neuronal circuitry underlying OCD (Schwartz et al., 1992). Abnormities in specific areas of the fronto-striatal system cause changes of adaptive responses to environmental stimuli, especially basal ganglia dysfunctions lead to repetitive and rigid behaviours (Bradshaw & Sheppard, 2000). MRI-scans of the basal ganglia in autism and OCD patients show enlarged volumina of the nucleus caudatus being associated with compulsive rituals and motor mannerisms (Sears, Vest, Mohamed, Bailey, Ramson, & Piven, 1999; Hollander, Anagnostou, Chaplin, Esposito, Haznedar, Licalzi, Wassermann et al., 2005). A lack of basal-ganglia-modulated cortical inhibition can lead to hyperactivity in the orbitofrontal cortex. As the orbito-frontal cortex is overactive, flexible behav12 Obsessive-compulsive phenomena and symptoms in Asperger’s disorder and High-functioning Autism ioural responses to thoughts and urges are impaired (Schwartz et al., 1992; Schwartz & Beyette, 1997). As the amygdala plays a crucial role in the modulation of anxiety and fear (Wand, 2005), metabolic changes in the amygdalae of autistic individuals can lead to phobias and anxiety (Amaral & Corbett, 2002). Also in OCD the amygdala has been found to be overactive (Steketee & Pigott, 2006, p. 55). Reduced amygdala volumes have already been associated with rigid and compulsive behaviours and narrow interests (Dziobek, Fleck, Rogers, Wolf, & Convit, 2006). Imbalances of several neurotransmitters like serotonin and dopamine are more or less involved in the pathogenesis of autism and OCD (GrossIsseroff, Hermesh, & Weizman, 2001; Steketee & Pigott, 2006, p. 59). Serotonergic dysfunction is a crucial element in the etiology of OCD (Baumgarten & Grozdanovic, 1998) and has also been reported in the literature about autism (Winter & Schreibman, 2002). Serotonin re-uptake inhibitors (SSRI) have not only been shown to be effective in OCD (Greist, Jefferson, & Kobak, 1995), in some cases they can also improve AOCP (Levallois et al., 2007) and other autism-related symptoms like social deficits and aggression (Howlin, 2004, p. 290). Additionally, family studies have shown mutations of the serotonin transporter gene in both disorders (Ozaki et al., 2003; Wendland, DeGuzman, McMahon, Rudnick, DeteraWadleigh, & Murphy, 2008). The property of the dopamine-agonists LDopa and amphetamines of inducing stereotyped behaviour (Ricciardi & Hurley, 1990) implies the involvement of dopaminergic pathways in stereotypal behaviour. Also the efficacy of dopamine-antagonist antipsychotics in reducing tic-like compulsions and some classes of stereotypal behaviour, can serve as evidence for the involvement of the dopaminergic system into autism (Howlin, 2004; Levallois et al., 2007) and tic-related OCD (McDougle, 1992), also the role of nicotinic acetylcholine-receptors both in OCD (Pasquini, Garavini, & Biondi, 2005) and autism (Lipiello, 2006) is being investigated. 3.3.3 Genetic similarities The occurrence of OCD in families can predict a genetic vulnerability for autism. In comparison with normal controls motor tics, OCD symptoms, anxiety disorders and depression run significantly more often in families of autistic patients, while there is evidence for the transmission of a broad phenotype (Bolton, Pickles, Murphy, & Rutter, 1998; Wendland et al., 2008). OCD symptoms in parents are often positively correlated with strong repetitive behaviours in autistic children (Hollander, King, Delaney, Smith, & Silverman, 2003), e.g. the obsessive insistence in sameness (Abramson, Ravan, Wright, Wieduwilt, Wolpert, Donnelly et al., 2005). Furthermore, OCD-patients display autism-like communicative impairment more often than individuals with depressive disorders (Bolton et al., 1998) and tend to have autism- related personality features in several cases (Bejerot et al., 13 Life Span and Disability Fischer-Terworth C. / Probst P. 2001). Research results from molecular genetics indicate genetic links between OCD and autism as well (Hollander et al., 1999; Ozaki et al., 2003; Fontenelle, Mendlovicz, Bezerra de Menezes, dos Santos Martins, & Verisano, 2004). 4. Obsessive-compulsive phenomena in autism 4.1 Classification According to DSM-IV-criteria (APA, 2000) Autism- related obsessivecompulsive phenomena (AOCP) comprise (a) the excessive involvement into one or more circumscribed special interests (b) the engagement in dysfunctional, compulsive rituals triggering anxiety of change when being interrupted (c) stereotyped and repetitive motor mannerisms (d) anxious insistence in sameness. OCD is characterized by the following features: (a) The occurrence of repetitive and intrusive obsessions and compulsions causing anxiety and/or discomfort. (b) The thoughts and behaviours are generally recognized as inappropriate (in the category OCD with poor insight symptoms haven’t necessarily to be experienced as ego-dystonic) (c) Individuals make an effort to resist the obsessions and compulsions. (d) The obsessions and compulsions must cause substantial functional impairment. Rituals are carried out to reduce anxiety and/or discomfort or to prevent a potentially threatening event (APA, 2000). Between AOCP and OCD-Symptoms there are similarities and differences as well (Hashimoto, 2007). Turner (1997) classifies obsessions and compulsions as belonging to AOCP. Although the involvement in special interests often has an obsessional character, AOCP- related preoccupations are not the same as the obsessions of OCD. Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright (1999) regard those obsessions as an ego- syntonic subtype of OCD (also see Fontenelle et al., 2004). AOCP are generally ego- syntonic as they are not experienced as inappropriate (Baron- Cohen & Wheelwright, 1999). Obsessional interests normally do not trigger anxiety or guilt, as they are often accompanied by feelings of euphoria (Jörgensen, 1994, p. 55). Other rituals in autism, however, can resemble the compulsions of OCD, as they are carried out to reduce anxiety or to prevent threatening events.According to Joliffe et al. (1992) rituals in autism serve as a structuring element in a world experienced as chaotic. Routine, the recognition of regular patterns, temporal structuring and rituals can help to reduce anxiety and feelings of confusion triggered by sensory overload (see Howlin, 2004, p. 137). The idiosyncratic stereotypies of autism, reaching from simple reflexlike actions to complex movement patterns sometimes resemble tic- related OCD symptoms (Rasmussen & Eisen, 1992). Stereotypies are often triggered by certain stimuli (Gritti, Bove, Di Sarno, D’Addio, Chiapparo, & 14 Obsessive-compulsive phenomena and symptoms in Asperger’s disorder and High-functioning Autism Bove, 2003) and may indicate distress or hidden anxieties (Howlin, 2004, p. 144). AOCP are not necessarily associated with functional impairment, although this is often the case. 4.2 Autism-related obsessive-compulsive phenomena (AOCP) According to Lam, Bodfish and Piven (2008) there is considerable structure within repetitive behaviors in autism being differentially related to subject characteristics and familiality. Using exploratory factor analysis the authors identified three distinct factors related to AOCP: (a) repetitive motor behaviors (b) insistence on sameness and (c) circumscribed interests . “Repetitive motor behaviors” were found to be associated with a variety of subject characteristics such as IQ or age. Like “insistence of sameness” they are associated with social and communication impairments, whereas circumscribed interests appeared to be independent of subject characteristics. Based on sib-pair correlations, insistence on sameness and circumscribed interests and circumscribed interests appear to be familial. According to Lam et al. (2008) the presence of multiple forms of AOCP in an individual is associated with more impairment in the social and communication domains, suggesting a more severe autistic disorder. 4.2.1 Circumscribed Interests Circumscribed interests often appearing obsessional and bizarre, are common in AD/HFA.The preoccupation often becomes so dominant in the individuals’ lives that other interests and duties are neglected (Jörgensen, 1994; Howlin, 2004). Special interests can be linked with special abilities and differ from the other categories of AOCP (South et al., 2005; Lam et al., 2008). Individuals being fascinated by the subject of their interest, commonly acquire great knowledge in a special, circumscribed field mostly having to do with technical aspects of objects and things (Baron-Cohen & Wheelwright, 1999). One individual collects exact information about the organization of seat systems in trains, another memorizes exactly timetables of arriving and departing planes and a third one may know in detail all types of vacuum cleaners. The interests are often dysfunctional, senseless and without any connection to a certain purpose, while in other cases autistic people can use their knowledge and talents in a job (see Asperger, 1944/1991; Jörgensen, 1994; Howlin, 2004). 4.2.2 Stereotypies Stereotyped movements (Bodfish, Symons, Parker, & Lewis, 2000) occur more frequently in autistic individuals than in those with other developmental disorders. Stereotypies like gazing at objects and fingers rocking, finger twitching or self-injuring behaviours are frequently triggered by minimal changes in environment (Goldman, Wang, Salgado, Greene, Kim, & Rapin, 2009), daily routine, feelings of emptiness, boredom or inadequate 15 Life Span and Disability Fischer-Terworth C. / Probst P. stimulation (Kennedy et al., 2000). Stereotyped behaviour can have multiple functions (Cunningham & Schreibman, 2008), often being associated with “narrowing or widening the autistic barrier” (Gritti et al., 2003). The behaviours and movements which tend to exacerbate under conditions of stress, can have sensory or social function (Cunningham & Schreibman, 2008), sometimes they can be anxiety-or tension-reducing (Howlin, 2004, pp. 144) or reveal inner conflicts (Tinbergen & Tinbergen, 1984, p. 86). In other cases stereotyped movements serve to avoid managing tasks or to arouse the attention of others. Self-injurious behaviour can have multiple functions, e.g. the one of self- stimulation in individuals who only “feel themselves” when experiencing physical pain (Kennedy et al., 2000, p. 560). 4.3 Transition from AOCP to obsessions and compulsions AOCP tend to become more complex with growing age, especially when there is a high intellectual potential. In the research literature there are descriptions of cases showing mixtures and transitions between AOCP and typical OCD-symptoms. The permanent verbalization of special interests often includes excessive ruminating about the special topic, where euphoria can change into anxiety (Howlin, 2004; Fontenelle et al., 2004). The excessive collecting and hoarding of objects like stones, CDs or books is a characteristic activity of autistic individuals. Collections are often arranged in elaborate catalogues containing precise numbering and data systems (Howlin, 2004, p. 141). The collecting passion of autistic individuals can convert into OCD-specific hoarding compulsions, when collecting and hoarding is associated with distress for the individual and/or the family members. Apartments of hoarders are often overloaded with useless objects towards which the individual is emotionally attached to in an unusual intensity. Patients with compulsive hoarding are extremely anxious to throw things away and feel excessive responsibility for keeping them (Schwartz & Beyette, 1997, p. 29). Fascination and anxiety can also be entangled in regard to obsessional interests. Objects being primarily fascinating can turn into objects eliciting phobic anxiety and obsessive fears (examples see Howlin, 2004, pp. 145). Box No. 1: Primarily fascinating objects trigger obsessions and phobic anxiety An intelligent autistic boy, who had been fascinated with all aspects of electricity for many years, had been experimenting with electrical equipment for many hours daily. Years later he developed intense and excessive fears and obsessions related to electricity, triggered by his parents’ warnings concerning his dangerous preoccupation. He then refused to touch any electrical appliance without excessively and repeatedly having checked their safety before. With the exacerbation of his obsessional fears he finally wasn’t able to use kitchen and bathroom because he feared causing an accident bringing electricity into touch with water. 16 Obsessive-compulsive phenomena and symptoms in Asperger’s disorder and High-functioning Autism This boy, whose autism- related special interest converted into an OCDsymptom, developed an electricity obsession associated with compulsive checking and typical avoidance behaviour which would require the diagnosis of autism with concomitant OCD. The keeping of fixed times, rituals and ordering principles is an essential part of life in autistic individuals. Problems are likely to occur when life conditions change and important activities get neglected in favour of ritualized behaviours (see Howlin, 2004; Box No.2). Box No. 2: Problems caused by the change of habits An autistic boy living in Australia had been fascinated by meteorology. Every day he had been checking the congruence of the weather forecast with the observable weather which was always “guaranteed” in his home country. After having moved to England with his family, he became increasingly disturbed about the fact that in Great Britain the real weather frequently differed from the weather predicted in the forecast. A young woman diagnosed with an autistic disorder insisted compulsively in keeping a certain order in her room in her parents’ house. Although living in an institution, no family member was ever allowed to move any item in the room because she had strongly forbidden any kind of minimal change there. Like in many cases of OCD, a need for absolute completeness, sameness and symmetry gets evident in both examples of Box No. 2 (Rasmussen & Eisen, 1992). The boy’s behaviour, a typical autism-related special interest accompanied by a strong ritualization, also displays features of a checking compulsion. The interruption or the incompleteness of rituals causes significant distress in autistic individuals and is also characteristic for OCD. The intense fear of change underlying these rituals is a typically autistic feature also applying to several OCD- symptoms. The woman’s behaviour has typical elements of an ordering compulsion accompanying the autism- related insistence on sameness. In both cases OCD symptoms and autistic features go hand in hand (Bejerot et al., 2001; Bejerot, 2007). 4.4 Obsessions and compulsions in autistic disorders 4.4.1 Examples In the autism literature there are descriptions of several obsessive thoughts and compulsive behaviours meeting the DSM- IV- criteria for OCD-related obsessions and compulsions (see Howlin, 2004; Box No.3). 17 Life Span and Disability Fischer-Terworth C. / Probst P. Box No. 3: Obsessions and compulsions A very gifted young woman had autistic symptoms being hardly recognizable for other people. The clinical picture was dominated by compulsive rituals she had to carry out in her room during night- time because her parents had set limits to their daily performance in other rooms. She always had to pull the curtains from left to right and vice versa as often as they were exactly in the right position. She was also compelled to remove even the smallest dust particles on the floor. A boy with an autistic disorder developed the typical aggressive obsession to leave a shop without having paid. He feared to have stolen something without having recognized it. Even for the smallest expenses he insisted in receiving the bill to be absolutely sure that he had paid. He systematically collected all the bills because he feared being arrested as a thief.When he once could not find one of his collected bills, he became very anxious and agitated. The woman in the first example displays obsessions and compulsions related to symmetry and cleanliness. She had to engage in ritualized adjusting the curtains and cleaning the floor till she experienced a typical OCD-related just right-feeling of perfect symmetry, completeness and cleanliness (see Rasmussen & Eisen, 1992). The boy described in the second example suffered from a typical aggressive obsession associated with the fear of becoming guilty. The obsession is neutralized by compulsive reassuring and checking behaviours. The symptom again displays a kind of dissociation experience because like many compulsive checkers the boy could not trust his own perception and memory. These “black outs”, the dark spot in the experience of OCD-patients, lead to feelings of permanent insecurity, incompleteness and doubt (Hoffmann & Hofmann, 2004). The type of symptoms described in Box. No. 4 might also occur in cases of OCD not related to autistic disorders. Green et al. (2000) described a boy with AS who developed an obsessive fear of vomiting. He ruminated about how to avoid people who might vomit in front of him because of having certain diseases. Although he didn’t regard his fear as completely unreasonable, he felt the wish to overcome his obsession because of the substantial impairment it caused (Fontenelle et al., 2004; Cath et al., 2008). In this case the classification as a comorbid OCDsymptom belonging to the category OCD with poor insight (DSM-IV: APA, 2000) seems to be appropriate. 4.4.2 Clinical trials (a) McDougle et al. (1995) studied repetitive thoughts and behaviours in adults with a primary diagnosis of autistic disorder and compared them with the obsessions and compulsions of OCD, measured with the YaleBrown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale (YBOCS). Individuals with OCD had more washing, checking and counting compulsions, whereas autistic probands displayed a greater frequency of com18 Obsessive-compulsive phenomena and symptoms in Asperger’s disorder and High-functioning Autism pulsive questioning, telling, hoarding, ordering and touching as well as selfinjurious behaviours. The autistic patients had significantly less aggressive, religious, sexual and somatic obsessions than the OCD-patients. A subgroup of seven YBOCS symptom variables could be identified which reliably predicted belonging to the autism group. The authors concluded that repetitive thoughts, behaviours and activities in autism significantly differ from the obsessions and compulsions displayed in the OCD-group (McDougle et al., 1995). (b) Zandt et al. (2006) compared repetitive behaviours of children with HFA and those displayed by children with OCD. Both groups reported more obsessions and compulsions than a typically developing comparison group, whereas children with OCD reported more obsessions and compulsions than children with HFA. Levels of sameness behaviour and repetitive movements in the clinical groups were similar, whereas children with OCD engaged in more repetitive behaviour focussed on routines and rituals. According to the authors, types of obsessions and compulsions tended to be less sophisticated in children with HFA than in those with OCD (Zandt et al., 2006). (c) Russell et al. (2005) compared obsessive- compulsive thoughts and behaviors in AS and HFA-patients (n= 40) with obsessions and compulsions experienced by OCD-Patients (n= 45); 10 (25%) participants were diagnosed with AS/HFA plus comorbid OCD (AS/HFA+OCD). AS/HFAparticipants with an average IQ frequently displayed intrusive, time- consuming obsessions and compulsions causing significant distress. Only somatic obsessions, repeating compulsions and checking compulsions occured significantly more often in the OCD-group. Comparing the OCD+AS/HFA-group with the OCD-group it became evident that somatic obsessions occured significantly more commonly in the OCD-group, whereas more AS/HFA+OCD-patients suffered from sexual obsessions (Russell et al., 2005). As 50 % of the participants in the study of McDougle et al. (1995) (a) had IQ levels below average, many of them may not have been able to communicate about all their OCD-symptoms, especially they couldn’t report obsessions (Russell et al., 2005). The results of Zandt et al. (2006) (b) and Russell et al. (2005) (c) reveal that individuals with AS/HFA often have obsessions and compulsions according to DSM-IV-criteria. 4.5 Treatment 4.5.1 Obsessive-compulsive disorder The most effective treatment of OCD is cognitive behavioural therapy (Rosa-Alcàzar, Sanchez-Meca, Gómez-Conesa, & Marin-Martinez, 2008), often in combination with antidepressants selectively blocking Serotonin re-uptake (SSRI; Choi, 2009). The therapeutic rationale of CBT is domi19 Life Span and Disability Fischer-Terworth C. / Probst P. nated by the techniques of exposure and response prevention (ERP) combined with cognitive interventions (Hand, 1992; Salkovskis et al., 1997). After identifying an obsession or a compulsive urge as a symptom of OCD on the cognitive level, a patient with a washing compulsion e.g. decides voluntarily to touch ”contaminated objects” (exposure) and then refrains from washing his hands for e.g. 30 minutes or longer (response prevention). He can now realize that anxiety and discomfort gradually decrease with each exposure, although he does not engage in compulsive washing. By reducing anxiety and the intensity of compulsive urges, SSRIs help the individual to resist obsessions compulsions and to shift attention to more adaptive behaviours. CBT helps gradually to disengage OCD-related neuronal circuits and to activate neuronal pathways associated with adaptive behaviours (Schwartz et al., 1992; Schwartz & Beyette, 1997). As family members are often strongly involved into OCD (Probst et al., 1979; Livingston- van Noppen, Rasmussen, Eisen, & McCartney, 1990), in many cases additional family treatment is required (Steketee & Pigott, 2006, pp. 46), especially in children and adolescents. 4.5.2 Treatment approaches to Autism-related obsessive compulsive phenomena Different behavioural and pharmacological interventions being effective for OCD have been shown to be helpful in reducing AOCP and OCDsymptoms in autistic individuals. On the medication level SSRIs are sometimes efficient (Kolevzon, Mathewson, & Hollander, 2006)., also the co-administration or monotherapy with atypical antipsychotics has been shown to be helpful, especially in cases of strong tic- like stereotypies (Malone & Waheed, 2009). CBT interventions include the OCD standard technique of gradual exposure and response prevention (Lindley, Marks, Philipott, & Snowden 1977), which can be individually adapted to autistic individuals, like as shown by the successful intervention for an 12-year old AD patient with comorbid OCD (Lehmkuhl et al., 2008). Methods which also may be effective are habit reversal, behavioural refocusing and individual variants of thought stopping (Howlin, 2004, p. 147). Nilsson and Ekselius (2009) even have shown the effectiveness of repeated electroconvulsive therapy for an 38-year-old AD patient with refractory OCD-symptoms. 5. Conclusion Individuals with autistic disorders frequently display obsessions and compulsions co-occurring with Autism- related obsessive- compulsive phenomena (AOCP). In comparison with OCD-symptoms, AOCP are primarily ego- syntonic, serve with different functions for the individuals and don’t elicit anxiety and discomfort. However, obsessive-compulsive phenomena 20 Obsessive-compulsive phenomena and symptoms in Asperger’s disorder and High-functioning Autism in OCD and autism cannot be dichotomized in terms of ego-syntony and ego-dystony (Fontenelle et al., 2004; Ivarsson & Melin, 2008). The lack of insight or poor insight (DSM-IV; APA, 2000) into the senselessness of obsessive-compulsive phenomena is not a specific feature of AOCP. It is also seen in obsessive compulsive personality disorder (Rasmussen & Eisen, 1992) and OCD-symptoms related to schizotypical disorders belonging to the borderline between obsessions and delusions (Sobin et al., 2000; Bejerot, 2007). Although primarily being ego-syntonic, AOCP-related special interests can have a deep impact on family life, e.g. when family members are tyrannized being forced to excessively participate in ritualized special interests. Autism with comorbid OCD-symptoms or a “mixture” of special-interests and OCD-symptoms, moving on a continuum between fascination and anxiety, can cause significant distress in patients and family members. AS/HFA-individuals having higher cognitive and linguistic abilities are able to verbalize typical obsessions, associated emotions, magical beliefs and the degree of insight. Further studies must reveal which obsessional and compulsive thoughts and behaviors in autistic disorders have to be regarded symptoms of comorbid OCD. Clinicians must take into account that typical OCD- symptoms may not be an integral part of the autistic disorder. An exact differential diagnostic assessment is essential, as autistic patients with comorbid OCD may benefit from standard OCD-treatments (Russell et al., 2005) or individual treatment designs for autism-related OCD. Controlled trials have to reveal which patients may respond to individual variants of CBT and/or pharmacological treatment. On the level of differential diagnosis is has to be specified which symptom profiles might serve as predictors for a patient’s response to certain treatment strategies. References Abramson, R., Ravan, S., Wright, H., Wieduwilt, K., Wolpert, C., Donnelly, S., Pericak-Vance, M., & Cuccaro, M. (2005). The relationship between restrictive and repetitive behaviors in individuals with autism and obsessive-compulsive symptoms in parents. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 36 (2), 155-65. Amaral, D. G., & Corbett, B. A. (2002). The amygdala, autism and anxiety. Novartis Found Symp. 251, 177-187. American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV) 4th ed. Washington, DC: APA. American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and Statistic Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV) 4th ed. Text Revised (DSM-IV-TR). Washington, DC: APA. 21 Life Span and Disability Fischer-Terworth C. / Probst P. Andresen, B., & Maß, R. (2001). Schizotypie: Psychometrische Entwicklungen und biopsychologische Forschungsansätze (The concept of schizotypy: psychometric developments and biopsychological approaches). Göttingen: Hogrefe. Asperger , H. (1944). Die “Autistischen Psychopathen“ im Kindesalter (The “Autistic Psychopaths” in childhood). Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 117, 76-136. Asperger, H. (1944/1991). “Autistic psychopathy” in childhood (translated and annotated by Uta Frith from the textbook of Asperger, H., 1952, Heilpädagogik (Remedial education) which includes a slightly revised reprint of his original paper written in 1944). In U. Frith (Ed.), Autism and Asperger syndrome (pp. 37-92). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Do autistic children have obsessions and compulsions? British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 28 (3), 103-200. Baron-Cohen, S. (2004). The cognitive neuroscience of autism. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 75 (7), 945-948. Baron-Cohen, S. & Wheelwright, S. (1999). ‘Obsessions’ in children with autism or Asperger syndrome. Content analysis in terms of core domains of cognition. British Journal of Psychiatry, 175, 484-490. Baumgarten, H. G., & Grozdanovic, Z. (1998). The role of serotonin in obsessivecompulsive disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 173 (suppl. 35), 13-20. Bejerot, S., Nylander, L., & Lindström, E. (2001). Autistic traits in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 55 (3), 169-176. Bejerot, S. (2007). An autistic dimension. A proposed subtype of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Autism, 11 (2), 101-110. Bejerot, S., & Mörtberg, E. (2009). Do autistic traits play a role in the bullying of obsessive-compulsive disorder and social phobia sufferers? Psychopathology, 42 (3), 170-176. Bhardwaj, A., Agarwal, V., & Sitholey, P. (2005). Asperger’s disorder with comorbid separation anxiety disorder: a case report. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35 (1), 135-136. Bodfish, J. W., Symons, F. J., Parker, D. E., & Lewis, M. H. (2000). Varieties of repetitive behavior in autism: comparisons to mental retardation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30 (3), 237-243. Boger-Meggido, I., Shaw, D. W., Friedman, S. D., Sparks, B. F., Artru, A. A., Giedd, J. N., Dawson, G., & Dager, S. R. (2006). Corpus callosum morphometrics in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders (epub ahead of print). 22 Obsessive-compulsive phenomena and symptoms in Asperger’s disorder and High-functioning Autism Bolton, P. F., Pickles,A., Murphy, M., & Rutter, M. (1998).Autism, affective and other psychiatric disorders: patterns of familial aggregation. Psychological Medicine, 28 (2), 385-395. Bradshaw, J. L., & Sheppard, D. M. (2000). The neurodevelopment fronto- striatal disorders: evolutionary adaptiveness and anomalous lateralization. Brain and Language, 73 (2), 297-320. Boucher, J., Cowell, P., Howard, M., Broks, P., Farrant, A., Roberts, N., & Mayes, A. (2005). A combined clinical, neuropsychological and neuroanatomical study of adults with High-Functioning Autism. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 10 (3), 165-213. Cath, D. C., Ran, N., Smit, J. H., van Balkom, A. J., & Comijs, H. C. (2008). Symptom overlap between autism spectrum disorder, generalized social anxiety disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder in adults: a preliminary case-controlled Study. Psychopathology, 41, 101-110. Choi, Y. J. (2009). Efficacy of treatments for patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder: a systematic review. Journal of the American Academy of Nurse Practioners, 21(4), 207-13. Cuccaro, M. L., Nations, L., Brinkley, J., Abramson, R. K., Wright, H. H., Hall, A., Gilbert, J., & Pericak-Vance, M. A. (2007). A comparison of repetitive behaviors in Aspergers disorder and high functioning autism. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 37 (4), 347-360. Cullen, B., Samuels, J., Grados, M., Landa, R., Bienvenu, O. J., Liang, K.Y., Riddle, M., Hoehn-Saric, R., & Nestadt, G. (2008). Social and communication difficulties in obsessive compulsive disorder. Psychopathology, 41(3), 194-200 Cunningham, A. B., & Schreibman, L. (2008) Stereotypy in autism: the importance of function. Research in autism spectrum disorders, 2 (3), 469-479. Damasio, A. R., & Maurer, R. G. (1978). A neurological model for childhood autism. Archives of Neurology, 35 (12), 777-786. Delorme, R., Gousse, V., Roy, I., Trandafir, A., Mathieu, F., Mouren-Simeoni, M. C., Betancur, C., & Leboyer, M. (2007). Shared executive dysfunctions in unaffected relatives of parents with autism and obsessive-compulsive disorder. European Psychiatry, 22 (1), 32-38. Dziobek, I., Fleck, S., Rogers, K., Wolf, O. T., & Convit, A. (2006). The ‘amygdala theory of autism’ revisited: linking structure to behavior. Neuropsychologia, Mar 24 (epub ahead of print). Esterberg, M. L., Trotman, H. D., Brasfield, J. L., Compton, M. T., & Walker, E. F. (2008).Childhood and current autistic features in adolescents with schizotypal personality disorder. Schizophrenia Research, 104, 265-273. 23 Life Span and Disability Fischer-Terworth C. / Probst P. Evans, D. M., Leckman, J. F., Carter, A., Reznick, J. S., Henshaw, D., King, R. A., & Pauls, D. (1997). Ritual, habit and perfectionism: the prevalence and development of compulsive-like behavior in normal young children. Child Development, 68 (1), 58-68. Fisman, S., Steele, M., Short, J., Byrne, T., & Lavallee, C. (1996). Case study: anorexia nervosa and autistic disorder in an adolescent girl. The Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35 (7), 937-940. Fontenelle, L. F., Mendlovicz, M. V., Bezerra de Menezes, G., dos Santos Martins, R. R., & Verisano, M. (2004). Asperger Syndrome, obsessive- compulsive disorder and major depression in a patient with 45, X/ 46, XY Mosaicism. Psychopathology, 37, 105-109. Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford, UK: Blackwell. Frith, U. (1992). Autismus – ein kognitionspsychologisches Puzzle (Autism-a cognitive-psychological puzzle) Heidelberg: Spektrum 1992. Frommhold, K. (2006). Zwang und Schizophrenie. Fortschritte der Neurologie- Psychiatrie, 74, 32-48. Gillott, A., Furniss, F., & Walter, A. (2001). Anxiety in high-functioning children with autism. Autism, 5 (3), 277-286. Goldman, S., Wang, C., Salgado, M. W., Greene, P. E., Kim, M., & Rapin, I. (2009). Motor stereotypies in children with autism and other developmental disorders. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 51 (1), 30-38. Greaves, N., Prince, E., Evans, D. W., & Charman, T. (2006). Repetitive and ritualistic behavior in children with Prader-Willi-Syndrome and children with autism. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 50 (2), 92-100. Green, J., Gilchrist, A.,& Burton, D. (2000) Social and psychiatric functioning in adolescents with Asperger syndrome compared with conduct disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 279-293. Greist, J. H., Jefferson, J. W., & Kobak, K. A. (1995). Efficacy and tolerability of serotonin transport inhibitors in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 52, 53-60. Gritti, A., Bove, D., Di Sarno, A. M., D’Addio, A. A., Chiapparo, S., & Bove, R. M. (2003). Stereotyped movements in a group of autistic children. Functional Neurology, 18 (2), 89-94. Gross-Isseroff, R., Hermesh, H., & Weizman, A. (2001). Obsessive- compulsive behaviour in autism- towards an autistic-obsessive compulsive syndrome. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 2 (4), 193-197. 24 Obsessive-compulsive phenomena and symptoms in Asperger’s disorder and High-functioning Autism Hand, I. (1992). Verhaltenstherapie der Zwangsstörungen: Therapieverfahren und Ergebnisse (Behavior therapy for OCD: therapy methods and their results). In I. Hand, W. K. Goodman, & U. Evers (eds.), Zwangsstörungen: Neue Forschungsergebnisse (Obsessive-compulsive disorders: New research results) (Vol. 5, pp. 157-180). Heidelberg: Duphar. Hashimoto, O. (2007). Similarities and differences between the behavior of Asperger’s syndrome and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Nippon Rinsho: Japanese Journal of Clinical Medicine, 65 (3), 506-511 (Article in Japanese). Herbert, M. R., Ziegler, D. A., Deutsch, C. K., O’Brien, L. M., Lange, N., Bakardjiev, A., Hodgson, J., Adrien, K. T., Steele, S., Makris, N., Kennedy, D., Harris, G. J., & Caviness V. S. (2003). Dissociations of cerebral cortex, subcortical and cerebral white matter volumes in autistic boys. Brain, 126 (5), 1182-1192. Hoffmann, N., & Hofmann, B. (2004). Expositionen bei Ängsten und Zwängen (Exposure treatment in anxiety disorders and obsessive- compulsive disorders). Weinheim (Germany): Beltz. Hollander, E., DelGiudice-Asch, G., Simon, L., Schmeidler, J., Cartwright, C., DeCaria, C. M., Kwon, J., Cunningham-Rundles, C., Chapman, F., & Zabriskie, J.B. (1999). B lymphocyte antigen D8/17 and repetitive behaviors in autism. The Journal of American Psychiatry, 156 (2), 317-320. Hollander, E., King, A., Delaney, K., Smith, C. J., & Silverman, J. M. (2003). Obsessive-compulsive behaviors in parents of multiplex autism families. Psychiatry Research, 117 (1), 11-16. Hollander, E., Anagnostou, E., Chaplin, W., Esposito, K., Haznedar, M. M., Licalzi, E., Wassermann, S., Soorya, L., & Buchsbaum M. (2005). Biological Psychiatry, 58 (3), 226-232. Howlin, P. (2004). Autism and Asperger Syndrome: preparing for adulthood, second edition. London & New York: Routledge. Ivarsson, T., & Melin K. (2008). Autism spectrum traits in children and adolescents with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22 (6), 969-978. Jörgensen, O. S. (1994). Asperger: Syndrom zwischen Autismus und Normalität: Diagnostik und Heilungschancen. Weinheim und Basel: Beltz. Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbance of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217250. Kaye, W. H., Bulik, C. M., Thornton, L., Barbarich, N., & Masters K. (2004). Comorbidity of anxiety disorders with anorexia and bulimia nervosa. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161 (12), 2215-2221. 25 Life Span and Disability Fischer-Terworth C. / Probst P. Kennedy, C. H., Meyer, K. A., Knowles, T., & Shukla, S. (2000). Analyzing the multiple functions of stereotypical behavior for students with autism: implications for assessment and treatment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 559-571. Kolevzon, A., Mathewson, K. A., & Hollander, E. (2006). Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors in autism: a review of efficacy and tolerability. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 67 (3), 407-14. Lam, K. S., Bodfish, J. W., & Piven, J. (2008). Evidence for three subtypes of repetitive behavior in autism that differ in familiality and association with other symptoms. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines, 49 (11), 1193-1200. Lehmkuhl, H. D., Storch, E. A., Bodfish, J. W., & Geffken, G. R. (2008). Brief report: exposure and response prevention for obsessive- compulsive disorder in a 12-yearold with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38 (5), 977-981. Levallois, S., Beraud, J., & Jalenques, I. (2007). Are repetitive behaviors of patients with autistic disorders, stereotypies or obsessive-compulsive disorders? Annales Medico-Psychologiques, 165(2), 117-121 (Article in French) Leyfert, O. T., Folstein, S. E., Bacalman, S., Davis, N. O., Dinh, E., Morgan, J., TagerFlusberg, H., & Lainhart, J. E. (2006). Comorbid disorders in children with autism: interview development and rates of disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36 (7), 849-861. Lindley, P., Marks, I., Philipott, R., & Snowden, J. (1977). Treatment of obsessivecompulsive neurosis with history of childhood autism. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 130, 592-597. Lipiello, P. M. (2006). Nicotinic cholinergic antagonists: a novel approach for the treatment of autism. Medical Hypotheses, 66 (5), 985-990. Livingston-Van Noppen, B., Rasmussen, S. A., Eisen, J., & McCartney, L. (1990). Family function and treatment in obsessive-compulsive disorder. In: Jenike, M., Baer, L., & Minichiello, W. E. (Eds.): Obsessive-compulsive disorders: theory and management. Chicago, Yearbook Medical Publishers, 325-340. Masi, G., Millepiedi, S., Mucci, M., Bertini, N., Pfanner, C., & Arcangeli, F. (2006). Comorbidity of obsessive-compulsive-disorder and attention deficit/ hyperactivity disorder in referred children and adolescents. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 47 (1), 42-47. McDougle, C. (1992). The pharmacotherapy of treatment-resistant obsessive-compulsive disorder. In: Hand, I., Goodmann W.K. & Evers U. (Hrsg.): Zwangsstörungen: Neue Forschungsergebnisse. Heidelberg: Duphar. McDougle, C. J., Kresch, L. E., Goodman,W. K., Naylor, S.T.,Volkmar, F. R., Cohen, D. J., & Price, L. H. (1995). A case-controlled study of repetitive thoughts and be- 26 Obsessive-compulsive phenomena and symptoms in Asperger’s disorder and High-functioning Autism havior in adults with autistic disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 772-777. Militerni, R., Bravaccio, C., Falco, C., Fico, C., & Palermo, M. T. (2002). Repetitive behaviors in autistic disorder. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 11 (5), 210-218. Malone, R. P., & Waheed, A. (2009). The role of antipsychotics in the management of behavioural symptoms in children and adolescents with autism. Drugs, 69 (5), 535-48. Nestadt, G., Addington, A., Samuels, J. F., Kung-Yee, L., Bienvenu, O. J., Riddle, M., Grados, M., Hoehn-Saric, R., & Cullen, B. (2003). Identification of OCD-related subgroups based on comorbidity. Biological Psychiatry, 53, 914-920. Nilsson, B. M., & Ekselius, L (2009). Acute and Maintenance Electroconvulsive Therapy for Treatment of Severely Disabling Obsessive-Compulsive Symptoms in a Patient With Asperger Syndrome. The Journal of ECT, Apr 17 (epub ahead of print). Ozaki, N., Goldmann, D., Kaye, W. H., Plotnicov, K., Greenberg, B. D., Lappalainen, J., Rudnick, G., & Murphy, D. L. (2003). Serotonin transporter missense mutation associated with a complex psychiatric phenotype. Molecular Psychiatry, 8 (11), 895, 933-936. Pasquini, M., Garavini, A., & Biondi, M. (2005). Nicotine augmentation in refractory obsessive-compulsive disorder: a case report. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 29 (1), 157-159. Probst, P., Asam, U., Otto, K. (1979). Psychosoziale Integration Erwachsener mit initialer Zwangssymptomatik im Kindes- und Jugendalter (Psychosocial integration of adults with initial OCD symptoms in childhood and adolescence). Zeitschrift für Kinder-und Jugendpsychiatrie (Journal of childhood and adolescence psychiatry), 7, 106-121. Probst, P., & Hillig, S. (2005). Computergestütztes Training von Einkaufsfertigkeiten bei einem Jugendlichen mit frühkindlichem Autismus: Eine Einzelfallstudie (Computer-aided training of shopping skills in an adolescent with early childhood autism: a single case study). Kindheit und Entwicklung (Childhood and development), 13, 122-128. Rapoport, J. L. (1989). The Boy Who Couldn’t Stop Washing. New York: Penguin Rasmussen, S. A., & Eisen, J. (1992). The epidemiology and differential diagnosis of obsessive-compulsive disorder. In I. Hand, W. K. Goodman, U. Evers (eds.), Zwangsstörungen: Neue Forschungsergebnisse (Obsessive-compulsive disorders: new research results) (Vol. 5, pp. 1-14). Heidelberg (Germany): Duphar. Ricciardi, J. N., & Hurley, J. (1990). Development of animal models of obsessivecompulsive disorder. In M. A. Jenike, L. Baer & W.E. Minichiello (eds.), Obsessive- 27 Life Span and Disability Fischer-Terworth C. / Probst P. compulsive disorders: theory and management (pp. 189-202). Chicago: Yearbook of Medical Publishers. Ringman, J. M., & Jankovic, J. (2000). Occurence of tics in Asperger’s syndrome and autistic disorders. Journal of Child Neurology, 15 (6), 394-400. Rosa-Alcázar, A. I., Sánchez-Meca, J., Gómez-Conesa, A., & Marín-Martínez, F. (2008). Psychological treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder: a meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 28 (8), 1310-1325. Russell, A. J., Mataix-Cols, D., Anson, M., & Murphy, D. G. (2005). Obsessions and compulsions in Asperger syndrome and High-Functioning Autism. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 186, 525-528. Salkovskis, P., Forrester, E., & Richards, C. (1997). Der kognitiv-verhaltensorientierte Ansatz zum Verständnis des Zwangsdenkens (The cognitive-behavioural approach towards understanding obsessional thinking). In F. Hohagen & D. Ebert (eds.), Neue Perspektiven in Grundlagenforschung und Behandlung der Zwangsstörungen. Ein Solvay Arzneimittel ZNS Service. (New perspectives in basic research and treatment of OCD. A Solvay pharmaceuticals CNS service) (pp. 93-124). Freiburg i. B (Germany). Samet, D. (2006). Asperger’s syndrome: emotional and social implications: lesson six: social or emotional difficulties frequently occurring with the diagnosis. http://www.theaspergersconnection.org/courses/esimplications/difficulties.html, 08. 06. 06. Schwartz, J. M., Martin, K. M., & Baxter, L. R. (1992). Neuroimaging and cognitivebiobehavioral self-treatment for obsessive-compulsive disorder: practical and philosophical considerations. In I. Hand, W. K. Goodman & U. Evers (eds.), Zwangsstörungen: Neue Forschungsergebnisse (Obsessive-compulsive disorder: new research results) (pp. 82-101). Heidelberg, Duphar. Schwartz, J. M., & Beyette, B. (1997). Brain lock: free yourself from obsessive-compulsive behavior. a four-step self-treatment method to change your brain chemistry. New York: Regan Books. Sears, L. L., Vest, C., Mohamed, S., Bailey, J., Ramson, B. J., & Piven, J. (1999). An MRI study of the basal ganglia in autism. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 23 (4), 613-624. Sobin, C., Blundell, M. L., Weiller, F., Gavigan, C., Haiman, C., & Karayiorgou, M. (2000).Evidence of a schizotypy subtype in OCD. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 34, 15-24. South, M., Ozonoff, S., & McMahon, W. M. (2005). Repetitive behavior profiles in Asperger syndrome and High-Functioning Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35 (2), 145-158. 28 Obsessive-compulsive phenomena and symptoms in Asperger’s disorder and High-functioning Autism Steketee, G., & Pigott, T. (2006). Obsessive-compulsive-disorder: the latest assessment and treatment strategies, third edition. Kansas City: Compact Clinicals. Tinbergen, N., & Tinbergen, E. A. (1984). Autismus bei Kindern: Fortschritte im Verständnis und neue Heilbehandlungen lassen hoffen (Autism in children: progress in understanding and new treatments give reason for hope). Berlin (Germany): Parey. Tröster, H. (1994). Prevalence and functions of stereotyped behaviors in nonhandicapped children in residential care. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 22 (1), 79-97. Turner, M. (1997). Towards an executive dysfunction account of repetitive behaviour in autism. In J. Russell (ed.), Autism as an executive disorder (pp. 57-100). New York, Oxford University Press. Wand, G. (2005). The anxious amygdala: CREB signaling and predisposition to anxiety and alcoholism. The Journal of Clinical Investigation, 115 (10), 2697-2699. Wendland, J. R., DeGuzman, T. B., McMahon, F., Rudnick, G., Detera-Wadleigh, S. D., & Murphy, D. L. (2008). SERT lleu425Val in Autism, Asperger Syndrome and obsessive compulsive disorder. Psychiatric Genetics, 18 (1), 31-39. Wing, L. (2005). Reflections on opening Pandora’s Box. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35 (2), 197-203. Winter, J. M., & Schreibman, L. (2002). An examination of repetitive behaviors in autism and obsessive- compulsive disorder: brain and behavior similarities. IMFAR Orlando, Florida. http: //psy.ucsd.edu/autism/WinterIMFAR2002.doc, 04. 07. 06. Zandt, F., Prior, M., & Kyrios, M (2006). Repetitive behaviour in children with High- Functioning Autism and obsessive- compulsive disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Jul 25 (epub ahead of print). Zandt, F., Prior, M., & Kyrios, M (2009). Similarities between children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders and those with obsessive-compulsive disorder: executive functioning and repetitive behavior. Autism, 13 (1), 43-57. 29 Life Span and Disability / XII, 1 (2009), 29-40 Cognitive profiles of genetic syndromes with Intellectual Disability Santo Di Nuovo1 & Serafino Buono2 Abstract The study of similarities and differences in the cognitive profiles of persons with genetic-based Intellectual Disability is relevant to increase our understanding about the complex way in which genetic aspects affect cognitive processes. Genetic syndromes have been mainly studied with reference to withinprofile variability. The aim of our study was to compare cognitive syndrome profiles in order to detect those cognitive variables that better characterize each syndrome. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children was administered in a sample composed of 156 persons with mild or moderate Intellectual Disability, 94 males and 62 females, divided into four groups according to their genetic syndrome (Down or Trisomy-21, Williams, Prader-Willi, Fragile-X) The groups were paired on chronological and mental ages and levels of maladaptive behaviors. Variance analysis across syndromes, followed by a discriminant analysis, were performed for all the variables. Results showed that the delay in cognitive functions is higher in attentionconcentration and visuo-spatial constructive skills than in verbal skills. The genetic syndromes had different profiles, with a higher level for Prader-Willi than for Down and Fragile-X; the intra-profile disharmony was lower in Down and higher in Williams syndrome. Discriminant analysis allowed us to detect the best discriminating subtests in the classification of syndromes based on cognitive points of strength * Received: 4 March 2009, Revised: 29 May 2009, Accepted: 29 May 2009. 1 2 University of Catania and Enna ‘Kore’, Italy, e-mail: [email protected] IRCCS ‘Oasi’, Troina, Italy, e-mail: [email protected] 31 Life Span and Disability Di Nuovo S. / Buono S. and weakness. Our results supported the hypothesis that Intellectual Disability reflects the impaired functioning of a complex system in which some skills are damaged more than others. The question of “what discriminates better among syndromes” may be answered assuming a “modular” perspective of mind in disability. To analyze what abilities are specifically impaired in each syndrome, is useful to plan specific rehabilitation procedures. Keywords: Genetic syndromes – Intellectual Disability – Phenotype – Cognitive profiles Introduction Peculiar cognitive profiles of genetic syndromes, as well as intra-profile variability, have been the subject of current discussions. The syndromes can be represented on a continuum based on the global severity level of the cognitive disability: namely, from the lowest level of impairment characterizing Prader-Willi Syndrome, through Williams syndrome and, finally, Down Syndrome. On the same continuum, stability of IQ is also represented: more stable for Prader-Willi (Waters, 1999; Roof, Stone, MacLean, Feurer,Thompson, & Butler, 2000; Dykens, Hodapp, & Finucane, 2000), less stable for Williams Syndrome, while a progressive reduction of IQ level was found for Down syndrome (Hodapp, Evans, & Gray, 1999). As far as the variability of cognitive performances across syndromes is concerned, Down Syndrome showed less variability than Williams Syndrome, which appeared to be more heterogeneous (Porter & Coltheart, 2005). Moreover, in Williams persons, a discrepancy also occurred between verbal and spatial memory (Vicari, Brizzolara, Carlesimo, Pezzini, & Volterra, 1996). High variability was also found in Prader-Willi and Fragile-X Syndromes (Kau, Reider, Payne, Meyer, & Freund, 2000). In this latter, variability seemed to be correlated to gender-linked genetic features characterizing the syndrome. According to Cornish, Sudhalter, and Turk, (2004, p. 11), “the Fragile-X Syndrome profile is characterized by uneven abilities within and across cognitive domain”. In order to describe within-profile variability for each syndrome, many studies have focused on the differences between cognitive functions. For example, in Down and Williams Syndromes, opposite profiles seemed to emerge when matching the general intellectual levels: verbal skills turned out to be higher in the Williams syndrome, despite many atypical features in different language areas (O’Brien & Yule, 1995;Volterra, Capirci, Pezzi32 Cognitive profiles of genetic syndromes with Intellectual Disability ni, Sabbadini, & Vicari, 1996; Mervis, Robinson, Bertrand, Morris, KleinTasman, & Armstrong, 2000; Mervis, 2003); higher visual-spatial skills were found in Down Syndrome (Lanfranchi, Cornoldi, & Vianello, 2004), typically characterized by marked deficits in language development, usually affecting more phonology and syntax than lexicon (Rondal, 2004; Chapman & Hesketh, 2001). A few authors highlighted similarities rather than differences across profiles: according to Fisch et al. (2007), cognitive and adaptive profiles of children with Fragile X and Williams are “surprisingly similar”. Cognitive outcomes of genetic disorders leading to intellectual disabilities were analyzed from a number of different perspectives (Hodapp, 1997). According to a “total specificity” perspective (Flynt & Yule, 1994), each genetic syndrome has unique characteristics that other syndromes have not. For example, with regard to cognitive functioning, higher visual rather than auditory receptive abilities were found only in Down syndrome (Pueschel, Gallagher, Zartler, & Pezzullo, 1987; Chapman & Hesketh, 2001); Williams syndrome subjects are typically characterized by high-level language abilities, impaired visuo-spatial functioning and low mental age (Bellugi, Wang, & Jernigan, 1994); attention, executive functions and visuo-perceptual organization are usually impaired in PraderWilli Syndrome, as a consequence of deficits in frontal cognitive processes (Jauregi et al., 2007). In a “partial specificity” perspective, a few genetic disorders had a single outcome, differing from mixed-etiology intellectual disabilities. For example, slightly impaired sequential processing was found both in Prader-Willi and in Fragile-X Syndromes (Dykens, Hodapp, & Leckman, 1987). In this perspective, analysis of similarities and differences in the cognitive development of persons with genetic-based Intellectual Disability appear to be of most relevance. The above mentioned studies focused on each of the syndromes as well as on the comparisons between syndromes, or – in a few cases – on the study of the same function across syndromes: e.g., memory in Williams and Down Syndromes (Wang & Bellugi, 1994; Devenny, Krinsky-McHale, Kittler, Flory, Jenkins, & Brown, 2004; Vicari, 2004; Vicari & Carlesimo, 2006), expressive versus receptive vocabulary and speech production in Fragile-X and Down Syndromes (Roberts et al. 2007); and, finally, specific language functions across several genetic syndromes (Rondal, 2004). In order to answer the crucial question about the disharmony of intellectual functioning in genetic syndromes – namely, specific impairments in each syndrome – we compared different profiles by pairing samples on chronological and mental ages. Aim The aim of our study was to compare cognitive syndrome profiles as well 33 Life Span and Disability Di Nuovo S. / Buono S. as to use a discriminant analysis approach to detect those cognitive variables that better characterize each syndrome. Method Instruments Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (suitable for 6-to-17 age ranges, and persons with < 18 years of mental age) was administered, including 11 tests, divided into two subscales, namely “verbal” (Information, Similarities, Comprehension, Digit Span, Arithmetic and Vocabulary subtests), and “performance” (Picture Completion, Picture Arrangement, Block Design, Object Assembly and Coding subtests). Scaled scores were used for each subtest. We administered the Italian version of WISC-R (Wechsler, 1974; translated, adapted and standardized by Rubini & Padovani, 1986). The long procedure of sample recruitment (several years), due to the rarity of some genetic syndromes, did not allow us to use the most recent WISC-III version, which has only recently been translated, adapted and standardized in Italy. Tests were administered by qualified psychologists, specifically trained in assessing persons with Intellectual Disability. Sample The sample (n = 156, males = 94 and females = 62), was composed of persons with mild or moderate mental retardation since WISC-R scale and other cognitive tests may be reliably administered only to people with these levels of Intellectual Disability. The sample was diagnosed according to ICD-10 (WHO, 1992) criteria. Table 1 shows that differences in chronological age, mental age (assessed with age equivalent composite scores derived from intelligence and adaptive tests) and levels of maladaptive behaviors – measured with Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale (Sparrow, Balla, & Cicchetti, 1984) – turned out to be not-significant in the subgroups selected according to each genetic syndrome considered. The difference in composition of subgroups reflects the different prevalence of the syndromes in our regional context; for some syndromes almost all the cases existing in Sicily in last few years were recruited for the research. 34 Cognitive profiles of genetic syndromes with Intellectual Disability Table 1 - Genetic syndromes with Intellectual Disability. Comparison between means (± standard error) for Chronological age, mental age (in months), maladaptive behaviors (from Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale) Total m f Chronol. Age Mental age Malad. behav. 103 56 47 182.04 ± 9.39 81.04 ± 1.44 11.07 ± .75 Williams 12 9 3 210.00 ± 25.91 77.96 ± 5.04 10.43 ± 2.79 PraderWilli 16 7 9 196.44 ± 19.59 83.16 ± 4.32 14.50 ± 1.97 Fragile-X 25 22 3 180.00 ± 18.12 77.63 ± 3.36 13.75 ± 1.90 156 94 62 F=.43, p=.73 F=.65 p =.59 F=1.47 p=.23 Down Data analysis Variance analysis across syndromes was performed for all the variables. The comparison are displayed in Figure 1, and Table 2 shows means and standard errors for each measure. Figure 1 - Comparison of syndromes in Wechsler subtests (mean scaled scores) 7 6 5 4 Scaled Scores Prader Willi Prader Willi Williams Williams 3 Down Fr-X 2 Fr-X Down 1 0 Vocab . Similar. Inform. Arithm. Digit Span Compr. VERBAL SCALE Pict. compl. Block Pict. Arrang. Digit Symb. Obj. Ass. PERFORMANCE SCALE 35 Life Span and Disability Di Nuovo S. / Buono S. Table 2 - Means and standard errors for each measure, and results of Analysis of Variance across syndromes Information Down 2.13 ± . 20 Williams 3.10 ± .60 Similarities Arithmetic Vocabulary Comprehension Digit Span Picture completi on Picture Arrange ment Block Design 3.08 ± .19 1.74 ± .13 1.99 ±.16 1.99 ± .15 1.33 ± .11 2.97 ± .16 2.12 ± .15 2.07 ± .14 3.47 ± 2.67 ± 3.38 ± 3.13 ± 2.84 ± 3.74 ± 2.93 ± 2.26 ± Object Assembly Digit-symbol 3.00 ± .20 1.71 ± .15 3.45 ± .58 1.85 ± .45 .55 .37 .48 .44 .31 .47 .45 .41 Prader-Willi 3.61 ± .52 4.85 ± 3.55 ± 3.69 ± 4.21 ± 2.83 ± 3.93 ± 4.71 ± 3.57 ± .48 .32 .42 .38 .27 .40 .39 .36 5.65 ± .50 3.68 ± .39 Fragile-X 2.46 ± .41 3.40 1.66 2.99 2.87 1.23 2.94 1.92 1.89 F 2.85 p= .04 ± .38 ± .26 ± .33 ± .30 ± .22 ± .32 ± .31 ± .29 3.97 p= .01 10.66 p<.001 7.73 p<.001 11.87 p<.001 15.25 p<.001 2.33 p= .08 14.21 p<.001 5.61 p<.001 2.43 ± .40 1.77 ± .31 9.61 p<.001 7.41 p<.001 Overall, the syndromes showed quite similar trends, although the score levels were different and marked differences were found in some areas. In the Verbal subscale, persons with Down Syndrome presented with the lowest profile.Those with Fragile-X had the wider within-profile variability: marked deficits were found in the subtests tapping on attentional areas (Arithmetic and Digit Span), whereas verbal profile in Williams Syndrome appeared to be rather homogeneous. Prader-Willi Syndrome showed the highest profile, despite marked decreases in attentive subtests. Across all the syndromes, the highest score for Verbal profile was obtained in the Similarities subtest. In the Performance subscale, similar profiles were found, albeit differing in score levels. Lowest and quite overlapping profiles were found in Down and FragileX Syndromes, whereas improvements in Picture Completion and Picture Arrangement were detected in Williams Syndrome profiles. Prader Willi Syndrome shows “Performance” profile rather homogenous, with a peak in Object Assembly and lower mean scores in Picture Completion, Blocks and Digit Symbol. On the whole, the more relevant deficits were found across all syndromes – despite their different levels – in attention-concentration subtest areas, as well as in Block Design, which taps on visuo-spatial and constructive skills, typically deteriorated because of the neuropsychological impairment due to the genetic disease. A discriminant analysis was performed to find a linear combination between measures (Wechsler scores) that best classified or discriminated among the syndromes. 36 Cognitive profiles of genetic syndromes with Intellectual Disability In consideration of the number of variables entered in the analysis, a backward stepwise method was used (alpha=.10).The Wilks’ Lambda index to test homogeneity among groups (d.f. 1,3,152) was .76 (F= 16,67, p<.001). The incidence of each of the variables on the discriminant function is shown in Table 3. Table 3 - Discriminant analysis for WAIS-R subtests. F-to-remove and tolerance (limit: .001) Subtest F-to -remove Informatio n Similarities Arithme tic Vocabulary Comprehension Digit Span Picture completion* Picture Arrangement Block Design Object Asse mbly Digit-symbol Tolerance 4.41 3.57 2.38 4.21 2.41 7.55 0.44 8.34 4.48 12.04 4.54 0 .39 0 .38 0 .42 0 .25 0 .36 0 .48 0 .54 0 .42 0 .37 0 .60 0 .50 * Variable removed after stepwise procedure The best discriminating subtests in the classification of syndromes were: - Object assembly: as previously shown in the analysis of mean scores (fig. 1), Prader-Willi scored the highest, whereas Fragile-X the lowest. - Picture arrangement: higher mean scores in Prader-Willi than in Down and Fragile X syndromes. - Digit span: higher mean scores in Prader-Willi and Williams than in Down and Fragile-X syndromes. Picture Completion was the subtest with the lowest discrimination power, with similar mean scores across groups. The eigenvalue for the first canonical variable (the linear combination of the variables that best discriminates among groups) was remarkably higher than subsequent combinations (.90 vs .24 and .12), showing a higher variance in the first variable (72% of the total dispersion among groups). Canonical correlations between these variables and the groups (represented as dummy variables) were quite high: .70, .44, .30. In the “classification matrix” of the discriminant analysis, each case falls into a specific group having the largest value of its classification function. The overall percentage of correct classifications was high (82%). The high37 Life Span and Disability Di Nuovo S. / Buono S. est percentage was found for Down syndrome (88%), the lowest for Williams syndrome (58%) Discussion The study of genetic syndromes with Intellectual Disability is relevant to increase our understanding about the complex way in which genetic differences affect cognitive processes. Our study aimed to explore what is specifically retarded in each genetic syndrome, and to compare the profiles obtained from samples matched on chronological and mental ages. Overall, results supported the hypothesis that, in genetic syndromes, the delay in cognitive functions is higher in attention-concentration and visuospatial constructive skills (as in Block Design task) than in verbal skills, as it is the case of Similarity and Comprehension tasks. The genetic syndromes have different profiles, with a higher level for Prader-Willi than for Down and Fragile-X. As reported by Porter & Coltheart (2005), the intra-profile disharmony is lower in Down syndrome and higher in Williams syndrome, prevalently in “Performance” tasks. The highest variability was found in Fragile-X, thus confirming previous results reported by Kau et al. (2000) and Cornish et al. (2004). The reported prevalence of verbal skills in the profile of people with Williams syndrome (e. g., Bellugi et al., 1994; Volterra et al., 1996; Mervis, 2003) was confirmed for receptive and semantic characteristics (Similarities, Vocabulary, Comprehension), but similar levels of performance were also found in the Williams sample for some visual-based tasks, such as Picture Completion and Object Assembly. The expected highest performance in Trisomy 21 visuo-spatial skills (Hodapp, 2004; Lanfranchi et al., 2004) was partially confirmed: Picture Completion and Object Assembly were the highest points of the profile, although we found at the same level Similarities, that is a verbal task requiring receptive language skills. This confirms that lexical aspects of language are less impaired in Down syndrome; while phonological and syntactic aspects (more damaged according to Rondal, 2004) are not assessed by Wechsler subtests. Deficits in sequential thinking found both in Prader-Willi and in FragileX by Dykens et al. (1987) was confirmed in our sample only for the latter syndrome: Picture Arrangement task, requiring sequential processing of information, was a point of strength for Prader-Willi cognitive profile. Discriminant analysis allowed us to detect the best discriminating subtests in the classification of syndromes based on cognitive points of strength and weakness. Object assembly was the subtest that best represented performances in Prader-Willi syndrome; this is a task – similar to puzzles assembly – which 38 Cognitive profiles of genetic syndromes with Intellectual Disability requires an ability that is unusually high in this syndrome, and considered as a common feature of the syndrome (Dykens et al. 2000). Picture arrangement, that requires logical and sequential thinking, was particularly low in Down and Fragile-X Syndromes. Digit span, i.e. verbal immediate memory, positively characterized Prader-Willi and Williams, and negatively Down and Fragile-X Syndromes. Our results supported Detterman’s (1987, 2002) hypothesis that “intellectual disability reflects the defective functioning of a complex system in which some competencies may be disrupted more than others” (Vicari et al., 1996, p. 503). To answer the question of “what discriminates better among syndromes”, we need to refer to some modularity of mind in the field of disability (e.g., “category-specific deficits”: Santos & Caramazza, 2002), although it is not possible to assume that modules related to specific abilities start out either intact or impaired (Paterson, Brown, Gsödl, Johnson, & Karmiloff-Smith 1999). A mild and non massive modular perspective may be useful in order to explain differences in genetic syndromes, and to analyze in detail which abilities are expected to be impaired, thus obtaining a relevant information for planning early and specific rehabilitation interventions. References. Bellugi, U., Wang, P., & Jernigan, T. L. (1994). Williams syndrome: An unusual neuropsychological profile. In S. H. Browman, & J. Grafram (Eds), Atypical cognitive deficits in developmental disorders, 23-56. Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum. Chapman, R. S., & Hesketh, L. J. (2001). Language, cognition, and short-term memory in individuals with Down syndrome. Downs Syndrome Research & Practice, 7, 1-7. Cornish, K., Sudhalter, V., & Turk, J. (2004). Attention and language in fragile X. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 10, 11-16. Detterman, D. K. (1987). Theoretical notions of intelligence and mental retardation. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 92, 2-11. Detterman, D. K. (2002). General intelligence: Cognitive and biological explanations. In R. J. Sternberg & E. L. Grigorenko (Eds.), The general factor of intelligence (pp. 151-182). Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum. Devenny, D. A., Krinsky-McHale, S. J., Kittler, P. M., Flory, M., Jenkins, E., & Brown W. T. (2004). Age-associated memory changes in adults with Williams syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26, 691–706. 39 Life Span and Disability Di Nuovo S. / Buono S. Dykens, E. M., Hodapp, R. M., & Leckman, J. F. (1987). Strengths and weaknesses in intellectual functioning of males with fragile X syndrome. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 92, 234-236. Dykens, E. M., Hodapp, R. M., & Finucane, B. M. (2000). Genetics and Mental Retardation Syndromes.A New Look at Behavior and Intervention. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing. Fisch, G. S., Carpenter, N., Howard-Peebles, P. N., Holden, J. J., Tarleton, J., Simensen, R., & Nance, W. (2007). Studies of age-correlated features of cognitivebehavioral development in children and adolescents with genetic disorders. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 143, 2478-2489. Flynt, J., & Yule, W. (1994). Behavioral phenotypes. In M. Rutter, E. Taylor, & L. Hersov (Eds), Child and adolescent psychiatry: Modern approaches (3rd ed.) (pp. 666-686). London: Blackwell. Hodapp, R. M. (1997). Direct and indirect behavioural effects of different genetic disorders of mental retardation. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 102, 67-79. Hodapp, R. M., Evans, D. W., & Gray, F. L. (1999). Intellectual development in children with Down syndrome. In J. Rondal, J. Perera, & L. Nadel (Eds.), Down syndrome: A review of current knowledge (pp. 124-132). London: Whurr. Hodapp, R. M. (2004). Behavioral phenotypes: Going beyond the two-group approach. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 29, 1-30. Jauregi, J., Arias, C., Vegas, O., Alén, F., Martinez, S., Copet, P., & Thuilleaux, D. (2007). A neuropsychological assessment of frontal cognitive functions in PraderWilli syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 51, 350-365 Kau, A. S. M., Reider, E. E., Payne, L., Meyer, W. A., & Freund, L. (2000). Early behavior signs of psychiatric phenotypes in fragile X syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 105, 286-299. Lanfranchi, S., Cornoldi, C., & Vianello, R. (2004).Verbal and Visuospatial Working Memory Deficits in Children With Down Syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 109, 456–466. Mervis, C. B. (2003). Williams syndrome: 15 years of psychological research. Developmental Neuropsychology, 23 (1-2), 1-12. Mervis, C. B., Robinson, B. F., Bertrand, J., Morris, C. A., Klein-Tasman, B. P., & Armstrong, S. C. (2000). The Williams syndrome cognitive profile. Brain Cognition, 44, 604-28. O’Brien, G., & Yule,W. (Eds.). (1995). Behavioural Phenotypes. London: MacKeith Press. 40 Cognitive profiles of genetic syndromes with Intellectual Disability Porter, M. A., & Coltheart, M. (2005). Cognitive Heterogeneity in Williams Syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 275-306. Paterson, S. J., Brown, J. H., Gsödl, M. K., Johnson, M. H., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1999). Cognitive modularity and genetic disorders, Science, 286, n5448, 2355 - 2358. Pueschel, S. M., Gallagher, P., Zartler, A., & Pezzullo, J. (1987). Cognitive and learning processes in children with Down syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 8, 21-37. Roberts, J., Price, J., Barnes, E., Nelson, L., Burchinal, M., Hennon, E. A., Moskowitz, L., Edwards, A., Malkin, C., Anderson, K., Misenheimer, J., & Hooper, S. R. (2007). Receptive vocabulary, expressive vocabulary, and speech production of boys with fragile X syndrome in comparison to boys with down syndrome. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 112, 177-193. Roff, O., Stone,W., MacLean,W., Feurer, I. D.,Thompson,T., & Butler, M. G. (2000). Intellectual characteristics of Prader–Willi syndrome: comparison of genetic subtypes. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 44, 25-30. Rondal, J. A. (2004). Intersyndrome and intrasyndrome language differences? In J. A. Rondal, R. M. Hodapp, S. Soresi, E. M. Dykens, & L. Nota. (Eds), Intellectual Disabilities. Genetics, Behaviour and Inclusion. London: Whurr. Santos, L. R., & Caramazza,A. (2002). The domain-specific hypothesis: a developmental and comparative perspective on category-specific deficits. In E. M. E. Forde, & G. W. Humphreys (Eds), Category specificity in brain and mind. Howe: Psychology Press. Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1984). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. American Guidance Service: Circle Pines. Vicari, S., Brizzolara, D., Carlesimo, G. A., Pezzini, G., & Volterra, V. (1996). Memory abilities in children with Williams syndrome. Cortex, 32, 503-514. Vicari, S., & Carlesimo, G.A. (2006). Short-term memory deficits are not uniform in Down and Williams syndromes, Neuropsychological Review, 16, 87-94. Vicari, S. (2004). Memory development and intellectual disabilities. Acta Paediatrica Supplement, 93, 60-63 Volterra, V., Capirci, O., Pezzini, G., Sabbadini, L., & Vicari, S. (1996). Linguistic abilities in Italian children with Williams sindrome. Cortex, 32, 663-77. W.H.O. (1992). The ICD-10 Classification of mental and behavioural disorders: clinical description and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva: World Health Organization. Wang, P. P., & Bellugi, U. (1994). Evidence from two genetic syndromes for a dissociation between verbal and visual-spatial short-term memory. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 16, 317–322. 41 Life Span and Disability Di Nuovo S. / Buono S. Waters, J. (1999). Prader-Willi Syndrome. A Pratical Guide. London: David Fulton. Wechsler, D. (1974). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for children - Revised. New York: The Psychological Corporation,. Translation and italian adaptation by V. Rubini & F. Padovani, WISC-R Scala di intelligenza Wechsler per bambini - Riveduta, Firenze: O.S., 1986. 42 Life Span and Disability / XII, 1 (2009), 41-52 Genetic Syndromes Causing Mental Retardation: deficit and surplus in school performance and social adaptability compared to cognitive functioning Renzo Vianello1 & Silvia Lanfranchi 1 Abstract In this paper we reported some results of research carried out in Italy with participants with Mental Retardation (better defined as Intellectual Developmental Disability) due to genetic syndromes (Down, Fragile-X, Cornelia de Lange and Prader-Willi), evidencing specific conditions characterized by deficit or ‘surplus’ in reading, writing and maths performances, and in social adaptation respect to the intellectual competencies. In some cases the comparison was made also with respect to abilities of memory and language. Results suggested that the cases of ‘surplus’ are in our context more frequent than those found in International literature, and this may be due to the positive effects of the inclusion in mainstreaming classrooms of most pupils with intellectual disabilities. A debate on these issues, comparing different cultural and social realities, is welcome. Keywords: Intellectual Disabilities, Deficit, Surplus, Genetic Syndromes Introduction The progressive shifting from the expression ‘Mental Retardation’ toward the more adequate ‘Intellectual Developmental Disabilities’ was motivated by its even more frequent use in scientific literature and the presence of this expression in the name of different Associations (Luckasson & Reeve, 2001; Shalock, Luckasson, & Shogren, 2007). * Received: 27 February 2009, Revised: 20 May 2009, Accepted: 20 May 2009. 1 University of Padua, Department of Development and Socialization, Psychology e-mail: [email protected] 43 Life Span and Disability Vianello R. / Lanfranchi S. Moreover, the use of the term intellectual disability shows the shift of the researchers’ interest from the study of mental retardation in general, to the study of the specific profiles of many syndromes causing mental retardation, better defined as Intellectual Developmental Disabilities (Dykens, Hodapp, & Finucane, 2000). Comparison between Down syndrome and Williams syndrome cognitive profiles shows complementary strength and weakness, as regards verbal competencies (higher in Williams syndrome) and visuo-spatial skills (more efficient in Down syndrome) (Vicari, Carlesimo, Brizzolara, & Pezzini, 1996; Dykens et al., 2000; O’Brien & Yule, 2000; Vicari, Marotta, & Carlesimo, 2004). Besides research evidencing what the different syndromes have in common – e.g., deficits related to the “Central Executive” function (Lanfranchi, Cornoldi, & Vianello, 2004; Lanfranchi & Vianello, 2004; Lanfranchi & Vianello, 2006) – many other studies aim to search for specificity of syndromes cognitive aspects. We will focus our attention on results of research regarding the cognitive-behavioral and adaptive profiles (including school performances) of four syndromes: Down, Fragile-X, Cornelia de Lange and Prader-Willi syndromes. Down syndrome is the most studied, and the most frequent genetic syndrome. Different authors have underlined the specificity of the behavioral and adaptive profile: compared with the intellectual level, linguistic performances are lower, except for pragmatic aspects (Buckley, 1999;Vinter, 2002; Rondal, 2004a; Rondal, 2004b); deficits are found in short-term verbal memory and high-controlled working memory (Lanfranchi et al., 2004); while visuo-spatial short-term memory are coherent with intellectual level, and adaptation skills are superior if compared with general intelligence (Vianello, 2006). As regards the Fragile-X syndrome (the most frequent hereditary syndrome causing intellectual disability), literature has shown marked differences between genders, deficit in working memory and sequential memory, good simultaneous processing and adult adaptation (Saunders, 2000; Lanfranchi, Cornoldi, Drigo, & Vianello, 2008). A different profile is shown in Prader-Willi syndrome (well-known for the hyperfagia), characterized by preserved abilities in the visual-motor discrimination if compared to auditory-verbal one, in the visual more than auditory attention, in the integration of spatial more than verbal stimuli, in simultaneous more than sequential processing, in long term more than short term memory (Waters, 1999; Dykens et al., 2000). A peculiar profile was found also in Cornelia De Lange syndrome, with particular points of strength, in visuo-spatial memory, perceptual organization and fine-motor behavior, and weakness in attention and language skills (Fiori, Lanfranchi, Moalli, & Vianello, 2008). 44 Genetic Syndromes Causing Mental Retardation In most research an assumption is implicit, that inter-syndromes and intra-syndromes differences are mainly due to genetic differences. We will suggest a line of research complementary to this, evidencing the interaction between biological bases and environmental influence, i.e. poor or normal or enriched environment (according to Baroff, 1989), producing cognitive and behavioral profiles of persons with genetic syndromes The constructs involved are those of ‘deficit’ or ‘surplus’ with respect to mental age. The ‘deficit/surplus’ hypothesis. Education and learning can significantly influence academic and social abilities in individuals intellectual developmental disabilities. Applied research (Baroff, 1989, Vianello, 2008) has revealed the existence of two opposed, contrasting phenomena. Zigler and Bennet-Gates (1999), on the basis of research by Zigler and co-workers over a period of 40 years, found that individuals with mental retardation show, at a motivational and personality level more negative behavior in the presence of strangers, psychological dependence on adult figures personally-known to subjects, less expectation of success, more importance given to external motivation rather than internal. As a result of this, the person is less motivated to work, has less self-esteem, and self-efficacy. A consequence of this is the individual’s taking less advantage of personal potential, and is therefore in “deficit” respect to mental age (Vianello, 2008); that is, performances below expectations of cognitive functioning. The phenomenon opposite to mental age deficit is characterized as “surplus”.This issue has had very little literature dedicated to its existence. It analyzes how adequate educational intervention can facilitate above-average performance compared to typically developing children wuth the same mental age (or equivalent intellectual age resulting from intelligence tests). In this paper we summarize the results of some studies conducted in Italy in samples of persons with mental retardation due to the genetic syndrome (Down syndrome, Fragile X syndrome, Cornelia de Lange syndrome, and Prader-Willi syndrome), showing the typical situations characterized by “deficit” or “surplus” in reading, writing, maths, and social adaptation compared to their assessed intellectual skills. We specifically evaluated the variables: intelligence, assessed with the Wechsler Scale (Wechsler, 1974) or other measures less sensitive to educational influences, as Logical Operation Test (a simplified version of Logical and Conservation Operations test, Vianello & Marin, 1997) and “Correspondence and Function Assessment” (CFV, Vianello & Marin, 1998); adaptation (assessed by Vineland Scales); in some cases short-term memory and working memory, linguistic production and comprehension. 45 Life Span and Disability Vianello R. / Lanfranchi S. Deficit and “surplus” compared to Mental Age in Down Syndrome. Vianello, Lanfranchi, Moalli, Petrillo and Sestili inproving an earlier study conducted on ten participants (Sestili, Moalli, & Vianello, 2006) have analyzed the relation between intellectual level and academic abilities (reading, comprehension, writing and calculation abilities) respect to a group of 19 persons with Down Syndrome. Table 1 - Mental age assessed with LO test, and academic performance of 19 individuals with Down Syndrome (age 13-14), primary and middle school 2 M ental Re ading: Ability To Reading: Comprehension Writing Can't read Below 1st grade Can't write Below 1st grade = 4.3 Can't read Below 1st grade Can't write Below 1st grade = 3 4.3 Can't read Below 1st grade Can't write Below 1st grade = 4 4.3 Can't read Below 1st grade Can't write Below 1st grade = 5 4.9 4 th grade 3rd grade 2 nd grade End 1st grade ++ 6 5.0 Can't read Below 1st grade Can't write Below 1st grade = Instrumental 1 age 4.3 2 nd Calculate st st 7 5.0 Beginning 2 grade 1 grade Beginning 1 grade Below 1st grade + 8 5.3 End 1st grade Beginning 1st grade Beginning 1st grade Below 1st grade + 9 5.6 1st grade 1st grade Beginning 1st grade Below 1st grade + 10 5.6 1st grade 1st grade Beginning 1st grade Below 1st grade + 11 5.9 Can's read Below 1st grade Can't write 1st grade, Interm. = 12 6.9 Can't read Below 1st grade Can't write Below 1st grade - 13 7.0 2 nd grade 4th grade 1st grade End 1st grade + 14 7.3 Beginning 1st grade 1st grade Beginning 1st grade 1st grade, Interm. = st st st nd 15 7.3 End 1 grade 1 grade 1 grade End 2 grade 16 7.6 Beginning 1st grade 1st grade Beginning 1st grade. 1st grade, Interm - 17 7.6 End 1st grade End 1st grade End 1st grade End 1st grade - 18 7.6 5 th grade 4th grade 2 nd grade End 1st grade + 19 st 8.0 End 1 grade middle sc hool st 1 grade middle school 5t h grade rd 3 grade = + As reported in Table 1 three children with Down Syndrome show a deficit (see last column with one less mark) in academic abilities respect to Mental Age, assessed in terms of “logical thinking” through the Logical Operation (LO) test, while eight children show a “surplus” (+ sign) more in reading, less in writing, and the least or completely absent in maths. It is our belief that this result reflects a different role of “logical thinking” in the three academic activities. 2 We refer to Italian scholastic system. 46 Genetic Syndromes Causing Mental Retardation Table 2 - Intellectual level (Test of Logical Thinking), daily activities and socialization ability (Vineland test), in 8 groups of adolescents and young people with Down Syndrome Mean Logical Age Thought Test Daily Activities Socialization Mean Equiv. Mean Equiv. Mean Equiv. score Age score age score age GroupA 24.3 7.3 4.9 285.0 8.5 170.3 7.9 Group B 24.0 11.7 5.7 353.0 16.6 245.0 15.2 Group C 27.7 10.2 5.6 324.5 13.3 228.2 13.0 Group D 23.0 8.2 5.2 280.5 9.6 178.0 7.6 GroupE 18.0 9.7 5.5 272.5 8.7 175.5 7.3 Group F 18.0 8.6 5.3 276.0 9.4 205.0 10.6 Group G 19.2 5.5 4.4 222.0 6.1 138.0 4.3 Group H 18.7 6.0 4.5 189.0 4.6 129.5 4.2 OVERALL 21.6 8.4 5.1 275.0 9.6 183.5 8.7 As far as socialization is concerned, we can consider another study conducted in Italy (Moniga, Beschi, & Maeran, 2008), which presents results from a specific rehabilitative plan based on life experience aimed at facilitating “independent life”, involving eight groups of children with Down Syndrome. As is shown in Table 2, at the equivalent “logical thinking” age of five, there is superior social adaptability and daily capabilities. Considering other research carried out in Italy (Ferri, 1989; Ferri, Gherardini, & Scala, 2001; Bargagna, Perelli, Dressler, Pinsuti, Colleoni, Astrea et al., 2004), the results seem to confirm the following tendencies: - Up to the age of 11-14, social adaptation tends to reach a similar level to that of children ages 6-7 (and superior to cognitive abilities of one or two years). - At a higher age, progress has been registered, even if very slowly, up to ages 25-30, which allows for generally average performances for typical developing children of approximately 8 years. 47 Life Span and Disability Vianello R. / Lanfranchi S. Deficit and surplus compared to Mental Age in Fragile X Syndrome In a recent unpublished research we conducted with Elisa Moratti, on ten children with X Fragile Syndrome (mean mental age 5 years and 3 months, and mean chronological age, 12 years adn 7 months (ranging from 6 years and 10 months to 17 years and 2 months). Figure 1 - Cognitive, linguistic, adaptive, and academic performance profiles of 10 boys with Fragile X Syndrome 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 0 IN TE LL IG EN CE LI N G .C O M PR EH . LI N G .P R O D U CT . V ER BA L M EM . V IS U O -S P. M EM . CE N TR A L EX EC . R EA D IN G M A TH EM A TI CS A D A PT IO N As shown in Figure 1 also in this case, surplus relative to mental age is evident in reading performance while less so in maths. Greater still is the surplus (corresponding to adaptive capacity) if the comparison is made with linguistic and mnemonic abilities. Deficit and Surplus respect to Mental Age in the Cornelia De Lange Syndrome In a study conducted by Fiori et al., (2008) on eight children with Cornelia De Lange syndrome academic performance parameters tend to be superior to mental age assessed with WISC-R in two individuals from the four taken into consideration (see Table 3). 48 Genetic Syndromes Causing Mental Retardation Table 3 - Performance of 8 young people with Cornelia de Lange syndrome in the areas of: intelligence, linguistic comprehension and production, visuospatial, adaptive ability and academic performance Chrono- Mental A ge Langu age Language Visuo-spatial So cial Academic logic Age (IQ) Underst andin g Produc tion Ab ility Ab ilit y Performance 5.5 2.8 (50) 2.8 2.0 2.8 1.6 - 7,6 3.8 (50) 3.0 2.6 3.0 2.0 - 8.9 2.8 (<25) 1.5 1.5 - 1.5 - 10.10 6.4 (64) 4.3 4.3 6.0 4.3 6.0 12.0 11.5 (97) 12.0 9.2 8.0 9.0 12.0 12.3 4.6 (38) 2.9 2.7 5.0 3.2 15.4 1.9 (<25) 1.5 1.5 - 1.5 17.6 13.3 (76) 12.9 1 1.5 17.6 7.0 (surplus) 14.0 11.5 (surpl us) Table 4 - Performance of 4 adolescents with Cornelia de Lange Syndrome in three different tests (WISC-R, LCO, and CFV) that evaluate different aspects of intellectual performance, and academic performance Chron. Age / Mental Age or Equiv. Age WISC-R OLC CFV Academic Perfomance 10.10 6.4 4.11 6.0 6.0 12.0 11.6 6.8 6.6 12.0 12.3 4.6 4.5 5.0 7.0 17.6 13.3 7.0 6.6 14.0 Comparing through different tests the intellectual level of four of the children, in Table 4 we see tests differences in the estimated intellectual level. The test used in this study were WISC-R (Wechsler, 1974), LCO (“Logical and Conservation Operations”, Vianello & Marin, 1977) and CFV test (“Correspondence and Functions Assessment” test, Vianello & Marin, 1998). These results are particularly interesting if we remember that in the test result the least influenced by environmental factors (educative, social, linguistic) is the LCO test - the CFV and WISC-R tests closely follow. For four persons, academic performance shows great ‘surplus’ compared to “logical thinking” revealed in the LCO test. Probably this is due to wery high qual49 Life Span and Disability Vianello R. / Lanfranchi S. ity educational intervention, that improved individual skills.. The surplus respect to the CFV test is also important. These results also show a certain tendency to improved performance respect to WISC-R results. Deficit and Surplus respect to Mental Age in the Prader-Willi Syndrome Research conducted by D’Amato, Gasparini, Lanfranchi. Moro, Raffa, & Vianello (in Vianello, 2008) offers data pertaining to Prader-Willi Syndrome (table 5). An interesting result is the intra-syndromic variability, comprising individuals with normal intelligence (n. 1), Borderline Intellectual Functioning (nn. 2, 4, 8, 9, 10) and moderate to profound mental retardation. In adaptive behavior, assessed with Vineland Adaptive Behaviour scale, there is a conspicuous surplus in communication and daily skills. Also, in academic performance, both surplus and deficit situations exist. Table 5 - Chronological Age, IQ, Mental Age (assessed with LO test), adaptive competence (Vineland test), and academic performance (reading, writing, maths) in 12 children with Prader-Willi Syndrome. The symbols + and – indicate deficit or ‘surplus’ respect to mental age. Chron. Age IQ Mental Age 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 5.8 6.2 6.6 8.0 8.1 11.2 11.2 93 79 78 47 36 5.0 4.9 2.0-2.6 5.9 1.6-2.0 5.9 4.5 6.1 + 5.11 + 2.4 7.7 + 1.6 6.10 + 3.10 5.0 5.1 2.10 5.0 2.3 9.4 ++ 3.11 2.8 4.2 2.3 5.5 1.6 6.4 4.1 5.1 5.1 2.2 4.3 3.7 + 5.1 4.1 - - 8 15.3 70 10.0 10.10 11.10 + 12.4 + - 4 th gr ade, Elem. 3rd grade Elem. 9 17.0 76 11.11 12.1 16.9 + 15.10 + - 3rd gr ade. Middle + 3rd grade Middle + 10 17.8 84 13.6 13.5 15.10 + 11.3 - - 3rd gr ade, Middle 3rd grade Middle 11 19.3 54 8.1 9.4 + 10.3 + 14.1 + - 2n gr ade Elem. - 2nd grade Elem. - 12 20.0 51 9.8 12.1 + 16.11 + 16.7 + - 1 grade Middle Commu Daily Socializanica tio n Abilities tion Moto r Abilities Reading and Writing st 50 Maths st 1 grade Middle Genetic Syndromes Causing Mental Retardation Discussion An adequate interpretation of the results presented in this article must assume the existence of a crucial fact: our research did not include any “training” nor selection of participants. This fact is important because the data must be interpreted in the context of a “standard” reality, not of particularly trained persons with mental retardation. Surely, the existence of a “surplus” respect to mental age tends to be present in the studies aimed to evaluate the effects of training or direct intervention (Rynders, 1981; Byrne, Buckley, MacDonald, & Bird, 1995; Rynders, 1999) But is this also valid, in an international context, for the general population of persons who suffer mental retardation for genetic causes? We haven’t been able to gather convincing nor sufficiently organicallyrelated results for definitive confirmation. Comparing earlier research results (e.g. Baroff, 1989) reveal that, above all for reading-writing abilities, this “surplus” characterizes the Italian reality more than others, and the crucial variable may be the inclusion of almost all pupils with mental retardation in mainstreaming and not in “special” classes (much different than in many other countries). At the conclusion of this article, it seems we have formulated more questions than generated answers. The affirmation that ‘surplus’ in academic and social performance seems to be greater where academic inclusion of disabled persons is more widespread, is still a hypothesis to confirm, however it may be interesting and stimulating. We also hope that this paper can contribute to promote a comparison between diverse cultural and social realities so that we can better understand how a academic context fostering integration could influence disabled student’s performance. References Bargagna, S., Perelli, V., Dressler, A., Pinsuti, M., Colleoni, A., Astrea, G., Rafanelli, V., & Chilosi, A. M. (2004). Rapporti tra abilità linguistiche, cognitive e profili di sviluppo adattivo in giovani adulti con Sindrome di Down. Psicologia Clinica dello Sviluppo, 3, 459-484. Baroff, G. S. (1989). Mental Retardation: Nature, Cause and Management. Second Edition. U.S.A. Hemisphere Publishing Corporation. Buckley, S. (1999). Promuovere lo sviluppo cognitivo dei bambini con Sindrome di Down: implicazioni pratiche derivanti dalla recente ricerca psicologica. In J. A. Rondal, J. Perera & L. Nadel (Eds.). Down’s syndrome: A review of current knowledge (pp. 115-128 Trad. it). London: Whurr Publisher. 51 Life Span and Disability Vianello R. / Lanfranchi S. Byrne, A., Buckley, S., MacDonald, J., & Bird, G. (1995). Investigating the literacy, language and memory skills of children with Down’s syndrome. Down Syndrome Research and Practice, 3 (2), 53-58. Dykens, E. M., Hodapp, R. M., & Finucane, B. M. (2000). Genetics and Mental Retardation Syndromes. A New Look at Behavior and Intervention. Paul Brookes Publishing. Ferri, R. (1989). Obiettivi e linee operative nell’educazione del bambino Down. In R. Ferri, & A. Spagnolo (Eds), La sindrome di Down (pp. 71-84). Roma: Il Pensiero Scientifico editore. Ferri, R., Gherardini, P., & Scala, A. (2001). Prime esperienze di inserimento alla scuola superiore. In A. Contardi, & S. Vicari (Eds), Le persone Down. Aspetti neuropsicologici, educativi e sociali (pp. 260-271). Milano: Franco Angeli. Fiori, G., Lanfranchi, S., Moalli, E., & Vianello, R. (2008). Profili cognitivi e adattivi in minori con sindrome di Cornelia De Lange. In R. Vianello, M. Mariotti, & M. Serra. Esperienze e ricerche sull’integrazione scolastica e sociale. Volume primo. Rassegne e ricerche (pp. 27-37). Bergamo: Junior. Lanfranchi, S., Cornoldi, C., Drigo, S., & Vianello, R. (2008). Working Memory in Individuals With Fragile X Syndrome. Child Neuropsychology, 15 (2), 105-119. Lanfranchi, S., Cornoldi, C., & Vianello, R. (2004).Verbal and Visuospatial Working Memory Deficits in Children With Down Syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 109 (6), 456-466. Lanfranchi, S., & Vianello, R. (2004). La sindrome di X fragile: caratteristiche cognitive. Psicologia clinica dello sviluppo, 3, 437-458. Lanfranchi, S., & Vianello, R. (2006). Memory Profiles in Individuals with Mental Retardation Due to Genetic Causes. In J. V. Engels (Ed) Focus on Birth Defects Research (pp. 53-80). Hauppauge, New York: Nova Science Publishers. Luckasson, R., & Reeve, A. (2001). Naming, defining, and classifying in mental retardation. Mental Retardation, 39, 47-52. Moniga, S., Beschi, F., & Maeran, M. (2008). Formazione alla vita indipendente: progetto rivolto a ragazzi e giovani adulti con sindrome di Down. In R.Vianello, M. Mariotti, & M. Serra. (Eds) Esperienze e ricerche sull’integrazione scolastica e sociale. 2 (pp. 249-255). Bergamo: Junior. O’Brien, G., & Yule, W. (2000). Caratteristiche comportamentali delle sindromi genetiche. Milano: McGraw-Hill. Rondal, J. A. (2001). Casi atipici di sviluppo del linguaggio nel ritardo mentale: implicazioni teoriche. Giornale Italiano delle Disabilità, 1 (3), 3-13. 52 Genetic Syndromes Causing Mental Retardation Rondal, J. A. (2004a). Variabilità sindromica nel linguaggio delle persone con disabilità cognitive. Giornale Italiano delle Disabilità, 2, 3-10. Rondal, J. A. (2004b). Intersyndrome and intrasyndrome language differences? In J. A. Rondal, R. M. Hodapp, S. Soresi, E. M. Dykens, & L. Nota (Eds), Intellectual Disabilities. Genetics, Behaviour and Inclusion. London: Whurr Publisher. Rynders, J. E. (1981). Risultati del progetto Edge, un programma di educazione precoce per bambini con sindrome di Down e per le loro famiglie. In P. Mastroiacovo, J. E. Rynders, & G. Albertini (Eds), La sindrome di Down. Nuove prospettive medico-psicopedagogiche (pp. 55-70). Roma: Il Pensiero Scientifico. Rynders, J. E. (1999). Favorire la competenza educativa degli studenti con Sindrome di Down. In J. A. Rondal, J. Perera, & L. Nadel (eds) (1999). Down’s syndrome: A review of current knowledge. London: Whurr Publisher. Saunders, S. (2000). Fragile X Sindrome. A guide for teachers. London: David Fulton Publishers. Sestili, M., Moalli, E., & Vianello, R. (2006). Pensiero logico, lettura, scrittura e abilità di calcolo in ragazzi con sindrome di Down di II e III media. In R. Vianello La sindrome di Down. Sviluppo psicologico e integrazione dalla nascita all’età senile (pp. 151-152). Bergamo: Junior. Shalock, R. L., Luckasson, R. L., & Shogren, K. A. (2007). The Renaming of Mental Retardation: Understanding the Change to the Term Intellectual Disability. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 45 (2), 116-124. Vianello, R. (2000). Progetto MS. Strumenti e materiali per il potenziamento del pensiero. Bergamo: Junior. Vianello, R. (2006). La sindrome di Down. Sviluppo psicologico e integrazione dal- la nascita all’età senile. Bergamo: Junior. Vianello, R. (2008). Disabilità intellettive. Bergamo: Junior. Vianello, R., & Marin, M. L. (1997). OLC. Dal pensiero intuitivo al pensiero operatorio concreto: prove per la valutazione del livello di sviluppo. Bergamo: Junior. Vianello, R., & Marin, M. L. (1998). CFV. Corrispondenze e funzioni: valutazione. Edizione per la sperimentazione. Bergamo: Junior. Vicari S., Carlesimo G., Brizzolara D., & Pezzini G. (1996). Short-term memory in children with Williams syndrome: a reduced contribution of lexical-semantic knowledge to word span. Neuropsychologia, 34 (9), 919-925. 53 Life Span and Disability Vianello R. / Lanfranchi S. Vicari, S., Marotta, L., & Carlesimo, G. A. (2004). Verbal short-term memory in Down’s Syndrome: an articulatory loop deficit? Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 48 (2), 80-92. Vinter, S. (2002). Habiletés phonologiques chez six enfants porteurs d’une trisomie 21 âges de 4 ans, Journal de la Trisomie 21 (6), 26-41. Waters, J. (1999). Prader-Willi Syndrome. A Practical Guide. London: David Fulton Publisher. Wechsler, D. (1974). WISC-R Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children Revised. Firenze: Organizzazioni Speciali. Zigler, E., & Bennet-Gates, D. (1999). Lo sviluppo della personalità in individui con ritardo mentale. Bergamo: Junior. Zigler, E., Bennet-Gates, D., & Hodapp, R.M. (1999). Valutazione dei tratti di personalità in individui con ritardo mentale. In E. Zigler, & D. Bennet-Gates (Eds.), Lo sviluppo della personalità in individui con ritardo mentale. Bergamo: Junior. 54 Life Span and Disability XII, 1 (2009), 53-66 Increased risk of neuropsychological disorders in children born preterm without major disabilities: a neurodevelopmental model Filippo Dipasquale1 & Paola Magnano2 Abstract Over the past 30 years, preterm births have drastically increased and today represent 12.5% of total births. About 1.2% of preterm births characterize very preterm births (GA<32weeks) that, with very low birth weight (BW<1500grams), are constantly found as risk factors of unfavourable neurological outcomes in longitudinal follow up studies. Actually, also “late preterm” children (preterm born from 33 to 36 weeks of gestational age), normally considered at low risk for neurodevelopmental disabilities, are supposed to represent a population of children to be monitored. Previous findings of a general cognitive impairment in children born preterm have gradually addressed the assessment of more specific neuropsychological skills and pointed out the importance to follow these children up to adolescent age. The neuroanatomical prerequisite of an abnormality in frontal lobe development and the correlation with various neuropsychological dysfunctions (fine and gross motor disabilities, executive function and working memory deficits, visual-constructional and attentional dysfunctions) underline the interference of preterm birth with normal brain maturational phases. Though showing more demanding neurodevelopmental pathways than term peers, a large number of preterm children tend to functionally normalize in adolescence. The review supports the hypothesis of a neurodevelopmental model that can be at risk to influence dysfunctional neuropsychological outcome. Keywords: Preterm infants, Developmental neuropsychology, Executive functions, Minor neurological dysfunction * Received: 2 March 2009, Revised: 6 May 2009, Accepted: 6 May 2009. 1 Neurodevelopmental assessment – Rehabilitation Unit University Hospital “Vittorio Emanuele, Ferrarotto, Santo Bambino”, e-mail: [email protected] 2 Ph.D. in Psychological Guidance Science, University “Kore”, Enna. 55 Life Span and Disability Dipasquale F. / Magnano P. 1. Introduction A brief report by the U.S. Institute of Medicine, published on July 2006 18th, outlines health costs related to managing the growing morbidity associated with the increase of preterm births and unfavourable outcome in neurocognitive, behavioural and learning areas. They outline figures higher than $26.2 billion, with shares of $51,600 for each child born preterm. This report stresses the significant increase of preterm births over the past 30 years that, with racial differences, today represents approximately 12.5% of total births (I.O.M., 2006). Births under 32 Gestational Age (GA) and/or Very Low Birth Weight (VLBW<1500 grams) characterize 1.2% of all preterm births, and represent conditions that have been identified as the main neurodevelopmental risk factors in several longitudinal studies (Marlow, Wolke, Bracewell, & Samara, 2005; Lindström,Winbladh, Haglund, & Hjern, 2007). However, for some years, a growing body of scientific literature reports findings of a neurodevelopmental risk even for so-called “near term newborns”, better defined today “late preterm” infants, born between the 34th and 36th GA, which account for about 70% of preterm infants (Engle, Tomashek, & Wallman, 2007; Petrini, Dias, McCormick, Massolo, Green, & Escobar, 2009). Epidemiological data obtained from cohorts of children evaluated from 1998 to 2004 during a follow up at the corrected age 18-24 months, published by the Vermont Oxford Network (VON) – an international network planned among Neonatal Intensive Care Unit (NICU) in more than 25 countries around the world – reveal a higher prevalence (between 25% and 30%) of minor neurodevelopmental dysfunctions (delay in acquisition of motor skills and other various anomalies documented by a Mental Index <70 on the Bayley Scales of Infant Development), than major disturbances (infantile cerebral palsy, severe sensory deficits, microcephaly, mental retardation). In support of this investigation, several other studies in Very Preterm (VP) infants have previously documented a clear prevalence of cognitive disorders and neurological diseases, which appear to characterize the outcome in 35-50% at preschool age (Hadders-Algra, 2003). According to this report, data showing a higher prevalence of a supposed intellectual deficit in children born preterm have often recurred in the literature, variously highlighting a borderline Intellective Quotient (IQ) (Bohm, KatzSalamon, Smedler, LLagercrantz, & Forssberg, 2002), or behavioural disorders and difficulty in control of attention (Bohm, Smedler, & Forssberg, 2004) or other neuropsychological disorders related to memory and executive functions (Bayless & Stevenson, 2007). Very preterm and VLBW children are considered at high risk to underachieve at beginning of school years, lagging behind their full term peers, because of their bad performances in multiple curriculum areas with most prominent difficulties in math (Pritchard, Clark, Liberty, Champion, Wilson, & Woodward, 2008). 56 Increased risk of neuropsychological disorder in children born preterm without major disabilities Recent findings are more frequently documenting a tendency to therapeutic interventions or other assistance for delayed school employment even in Low Birth Weight (LBW<2500gr) preterm that have been considered at low risk, eventually showing different developmental pathways to normality from infancy to adolescence than their term peers (Reuner, Hassenpflug, Pietz, & Philippi, 2009). Therefore, if we consider that Cerebral Palsy (CP), commonly affecting extreme preterm (GA<30) and/or Extremely Low Birth Weight (ELBW<1000gr) in a percentage of 15-25%, has attained incidence of 1.53% and that behavioural and cognitive disorders occur in a prevalence of 30-50%, we should expect more frequent effects in the medium and long term on a large amount of children, even at low perinatal risk, not only in terms of physical and/or cognitive functioning, but also on their emotional and psychosocial adaptation (Moster, Lie, & Markestad, 2008). 2. Neurodevelopmental risk and neuroanatomical correlations During the last century, sophisticated genetics, physiological and neuroimaging techniques have concurred to develop knowledge on the mechanisms that govern a lot of brain functions. Over all, advances in neuroimaging make it possible today to document developmental changes in cerebral structure, and emergent data on brain growth in preterm born infants have recently suggested a correlation with neurobehavioural or cognitive outcome during school age and early adolescence. Among diagnostic methods with high sensitivity in predicting neurological outcome, trans-cranial cerebral ultrasounds allow us to accurately describe the characteristics of lesions in very preterm infants already in the first weeks of post-natal age (Leijser, de Bruïne, Steggerda, van der Grond, Walther, & van Wezel-Meijler, 2009a; Leijser, Steggerda, de Bruïne, van der Grond, Walther, & van Wezel-Meijler, 2009b). Neuroimaging findings in children born preterm are well documented. White matter abnormalities, referred to as Peri-Ventricular White Matter Abnormalities (PVWMA), have been related to unfavourable neurological outcome in preterm. This assumption is based on the evidence of a timespecific Central Nervous System (CNS) vulnerability, with a complex pathogenetic combination of primary destructive and secondary maturational and trophic disturbances (Volpe, 2009). Recent studies agree that insults to a particular area of developmental brain, the subplate, prematurely lying in the periventricular zone and that addresses cell precursors migrating to the layers of the cortex, could be responsible for neurological outcome. McQuillen and Ferriero (2005) have emphasized the selective sensitivity of subplate neurons to hypoxia-ischemic insults that occurs in the perinatal period. In preterm newborn, according to artery flow distribution 57 Life Span and Disability Dipasquale F. / Magnano P. at gestational age between 23 and 32 weeks, a hypoxic-ischemic insult is feasible at the level of the anterior periventricular white matter that, if not leading to characteristic focal lesion - cystic Periventricular Leukomalacia (cPVL) or Intra-Parenchymal Hemorrhage (IPH) - may alter the maturation of cortical structures and establish important adverse effects on function, involving supposed neurotransmitter or fine structural deficits. Findings on the characteristic motor disorders in preterm born infants have focused the importance of early diffuse damage to subplate neurons. To describe the mechanisms underlying disorders observed during the development of motor behaviour, Hadders-Algra (2007) has assumed that the quality of spontaneous General Movements (GMs) - that have been identified as an early marker of proper functioning or malfunctioning of CNS (Prechtl, Einspieler, Cioni, Bos, Ferrari, & Sontheimer, 1997) - should be probably correlated to subplate maturation. According to Edelman’s Theory of Neuronal Group Selection, Hadders-Algra (2000) describes a linear path of typical changes of GMs characteristics during early development, which overlaps with the maturation of subplate. During human subplate maturation from 23 to 35 gestational weeks (see also McQuillen & Ferriero, 2005), GMs show the typical activity - fluent writing and fidgety movements - of a normal motor development in the 4-5 months post-term period. Grading the quality of abnormality of GMs, a motor poor repertoire or clear abnormal stereotypic movements, observed during a possible generic hypoxic-ischemic insult, could depend on a altered reduced selection of functional groups of neurons with consequent decreased normal variability of motor development. The Prechtl’s method, when administered at 2-3 months corrected age, shows a very high sensitivity and specificity (over 90%) to detect normal cerebral functioning or a spastic cerebral palsy, but it does not have a high positive predictive value in estimating risk of neurocognitive dysfunctions at preschool age (Einspieler & Prechtl, 2005; Einspieler, Marschik, Milioti, Nakajima, Bos, & Prechtl, 2007). Other studies are currently orienting their efforts to determine their positive value in predicting neurocognitive outcome at school age and early adolescence. Currently, most of the studies agree on the evidence of a modified development of brain functioning linked to preterm birth (SoriaPastor, Gimenez, Narberhaus, Falcon, Botet, Bargallo, et al., 2008; Stewart & Kirkbride, 2008). Nosarti, Giouroukou, Healy, Rifkin, Walshe, Reichenberg, et al. (2008), using Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) with a voxelbased morphology technique in adolescents born very preterm, documented a decreased grey and white matter volumes proportionately to early gestational age and grade of neurodevelopmental dysfunctions attested by low scores in language and executive functions. According to previous study, Ment, Kesler, Vohr, Katz, Baumgartner, Schneider, et al. (2009) using MRI, report reduction in volumes of cerebral grey and white matter in a group of children between 8 and 12 years born very preterm. In normal group MRI 58 Increased risk of neuropsychological disorder in children born preterm without major disabilities showed a gain of cerebral white matter on the grey matter, regularly deriving from a thickness reduction of the last; this feature was not present in preterm children, in support of the hypothesis of a disorder caused by preterm birth to maturational trajectory of brain development, which is evident in late childhood and early adolescence. 3. Motor and neuropsychological dysfunctions In the past, findings of low IQ scores in very preterm born children have induced a growing interest in detecting neurocognitive disorders in other neuropsychological areas. A low IQ score does not properly explain specific deficits in neuropsychological functioning and often does not provide a clear comprehension of neurodevelopmental outcome in order to justify daily live disabilities or academic failures. Early studies are actually oriented towards an assessment of neuropsychological skills considered at risk in children born preterm at school age. Moreover, children with a normal IQ have been found to lack specific abilities in fine and gross motor functions, executive functions, memory, visual motor integrative abilities, visual perception, spatial skills and attention (Salt & Redshaw, 2006). 3.1 Fine and gross motor functions The relationship between mild brain damage and neuropsychological disorders can be checked in children who presented an overview of “clumsiness” (i.e., “clumsy child syndrome”). Over the years minor disturbances of motor behavior, predominantly characterized by motor un-coordination, abnormal posture and fine manipulative dysfunction, were variously classified as: “dyspraxia”, “developmental apraxia”, “minor brain damage”, etc. In 1994, a first international consensus meeting approved the definition of “Developmental Coordination Disorder” (DCD). In DSM-IV this definition states, in general, a group of children with normal intelligence and poor motor coordination, with no obvious signs of neurological disease such as cerebral palsy or neuromuscular disease. According to the American Psychiatric Association, this disorder affects “...significantly academic achievements or activities of daily life” (Hadders-Algra, Mavinkurve-Groothuis, Stremmelaar, Martijn, & Butcher, 2004). DCD has frequently been correlated to Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), language, learning and other various developmental disabilities: it does not necessarily represent a condition of brain damage. In 1979, Touwen proposed the term “Minor Neurological Dysfunction” (MND) in replacing “Minimal Brain Damage”, to define a syndrome which may not always be related to brain lesion. According to the neurological abnormalities found at Touwen’s exam – including assessment of posture and muscle tone, reflexes, dyskinetic movements, coordination and balance, skill in handling, and 59 Life Span and Disability Dipasquale F. / Magnano P. other minor neurological signs - two forms of MND are today recognized. The first (MND-1) is the simple one: it reveals minor symptoms, with abnormalities found in at least one, at most two clusters of signs. This form is associated with a moderate risk in recurrence of behavioural disorders (ADHD) and learning disabilities, and it should not represent a condition of strictly organic disease, but a dysfunctional developmental status that could be placed in the extreme tail of the normal distribution of values of a low, non-pathological, quality of brain functioning. This form of MND may have a genetic component to the base of an ill-defined predisposition of Central Nervous System (CNS) to mal adapt to any stress (such as prematurity, Intra Uterine Growth Restriction, mild to moderate perinatal asphyxia (Hadders-Algra, 2002), or eventual prenatal overload resulting from a mother’s suffered psychological stress during the evolution period (Touwen, 1979). The second is the complex form of MND (MND-2). It occurs more frequently in at risk preterm children, or in combination with conditions that could cause injuries to the CNS during neurodevelopment. Diagnosis is based on abnormality assessed in more than two clusters of neurological signs. The MND-2 is consistently associated with risk factors such as very preterm birth and very low birth weight, and shows features through minor to moderate disturbances of motor behaviour, with neurological symptoms that, in serious cases, simulate clinical picture of a slight degree of Cerebral Palsy, a borderline CP. In longitudinal follow up studies a quantitative reduction of symptoms in affected children from the pre-pubertal period is frequently observed, with a tendency to persistence of slighter signs belonging to the cluster of more impaired skills (Hadders-Algra, 2002, 2003). 3.2 Executive functions (EF) and working memory (WM) A growing number of studies points out the impact of Executive Functions and Working Memory deficits in children born preterm at school age and in adolescence. Executive functions (EF) refer to a group of interrelated processes that support cognitive and adaptive functioning, including the inhibition of acting in response, the appropriate shifting and sustaining of attention for the purposes of goal-directed actions. Most studies are going to confirm their importance in children’s developing social competences and academic performance (Blair, Zelazo, & Greenberg, 2005; Carlson, 2005). Mechanisms that underlie the complex functioning of EF have been strictly associated to Working Memory (WM) that essentially represents the ability to maintain the online memory information. Planning and operational skills, requesting attentive and memory performance, are consistent with a well functioning working memory. As reviewed in recent studies, Working Memory involves central executive functions that are linked to brain maturational functions of frontal lobes that, as accurately document60 Increased risk of neuropsychological disorder in children born preterm without major disabilities ed by neuroimaging techniques, are constantly activated during performances in skills as inhibition, verbal fluency, memory, strategic problem solving that have been classified as executive functions.Among tests of clinical assessment, two of main evaluated WM skills are inverse span in verbal fluency and in visual-constructional memory. A classical model of Working Memory functioning assumes that verbal material is maintained into a phonological loop and visual-spatial material into a visual-spatial sketchpad. Currently from a neurodevelopmental point of view, a central executive panel seems to operate during preschool age and early adolescence, and it should not represent a defined memory store, but an attentional monitor involved in tasks that require attentional shifts and processing of information. Employing multiple tasks, this functioning could require more efforts for attentional capacity and absorb most of the time to perform. In older children and adults, instead, phonological loop and visual-spatial sketchpad become independent from central executive panel, requiring less time for attentional processes. (Conklin, Luciana, Hooper, & Yarger 2007; Saavalainen, Luoma, Bowler, Määttä, Kiviniemi, Laukkanen et al., 2007). Another recent study has documented a developmental process relating to performance in cognitive abilities assessing Event-Related brain Potentials (ERPs) activation. In the early stages of skill acquisition, ERPs are widely temporally and spatially distributed. It seems that neural activation travels across multiple brain sites that communicate continuously in flux with each other. When a skill is mastered, temporal and spatial relationships stabilize (Molfese DL, Molfese VJ, Beswick, Jacobi-Vessels, Molfese PJ, Key et al., 2008). The old epigenetic Hebbian model (Hebb, 1949) to explain brain development may have to adapt to these more recent findings. Although early appearing of EF abilities during development is difficult to test, a recent study has attempted to document some abnormalities in infants born preterm during their first 2 years of life. A significant difference in executive functioning was found between born preterm infants and full term peers, but it obviously needs to be confirmed by longitudinal assessment and follow up at preschool age (Sastre-Riba, 2009). In assessing EF during development, various critical aspects emerge in outlining an interrelation with deficits observed in many other areas. Extremely preterm born children pay the highest price in terms of deficit in executive functions and motor disorders (Bayless & Stevenson, 2007). In a study on motor hyperactivity and ADHD, difficulties in impulse control, working memory and other executive functions have been confirmed in preterm children when starting school. Two of the most important predictors for executive functions were gender (male) and visual impairment; these data invite us to better investigate the visual function (Böhm, Smedler, & Forssberg, 2004). Marlow et al. (for the EPICure Study Group, 2007) evidenced a very high prevalence of impairment in visual-spatial, perceptual-motor, attention-executive, and gross motor function at early 61 Life Span and Disability Dipasquale F. / Magnano P. school age in extremely preterm without major disability (CP, sensory impairment). They assume that neuropsychological deficits do not depend on motor impairment, but motor and executive-function difficulties together make an important additional contribution to worse school performance. A positive aspect is that motor abilities can be improved by therapeutic or educative strategies (Marlow, Hennessy, Bracewell, & Wolke, 2007); as recently reviewed, early motor intervention programs show they positively influence cognitive outcomes in the short to medium term (Spittle, Orton, Doyle, & Boyd, 2007). Korkman, Mikkola, Ritari, Tommiska, Salokorpi, Haataja et al. (2008) have well documented the interrelation between neuromotor performance and grade of neuropsychological impairment. Children with slight motor coordination problems show a widespread neuropsychological impairment, while most important motor disturbances, as reported in children with neurological abnormalities and CP, are related to evident neuropsychological dysfunctions in attention and EF, and in manual and visual-constructional tasks.They conclude that, in spite of average intelligence, the degree of motor disturbances could predict neuropsychological disorders. Other studies concerning working memory have been conducted in preschoolers, but the evidence that developmental maturation of the prefrontal regions continues into adolescence validates prolonged follow up studies. Saavalainen et al. (2007), in a group of 16 yrs old adolescents born very preterm and without cognitive impairment, report findings of minor neuropsychological disorders regarding only spatial WM deficit. In a longitudinal study, 16 yrs old adolescents born very preterm without major disabilities showed a good outcome in school performance, if compared to their full term peers (Saavalainen, Luoma, Laukkanen, Bowler, Määttä, Kiviniemi et al., 2007a). Though results are encouraging in observing only minor neuropsychological deficits in very preterm born individuals up to adolescence, we have to argue that participants enrolled to the study according their gestational age, birth weight and Apgar score at 1’ and 5’ as the main risk factors, do not have a documented clinical picture or concurrent disorder. Authors properly consider that, in spite of the documented good school performance, individual history can enfold a specific impairment and that parental monitoring or remedial support may have affected positively the good outcome highlighting the role of environmental variables. 3.3 Visual, attention and memory dysfunctions Visual, attention and memory dysfunctions have already in part been discussed above as concurrent aspects of working memory dysfunctions. Visual cognition occupies an important role among major dysfunctions affecting children born preterm, but significant differences demonstrate that visual perception seems to be less affected than visual-constructional skills. These findings suggest a neurodevelopmental disorder 62 Increased risk of neuropsychological disorder in children born preterm without major disabilities in children born preterm that has been associated with specific dorsalstream pathway vulnerability. The dorsal-stream pathway is a brain area (projecting from primary visual cortex to the parietal lobe) that is specialized for processing spatial information and in visual-motor planning. The theory of dissociation between the dorsal-stream pathway and the ventral pathway (projecting from primary visual cortex to the temporal lobe), primarily associated with perceptual recognition of shape, form, objects and faces, has received strong support from both animal research as well as neuropsychological studies on patients with localized brain damage (Santos, Duret, Mancini, Gire, & Deruell, 2009). As already reported above, visual spatial and constructional abilities are variously interrelated to dysfunctions observed in motor and working memory performances Saavalainen et al. (2007). Attention deficits are commonly associated to ADHD that has frequently been reported in very preterm born children (Bayless & Stevenson, 2007; Conklin, Luciana, Hooper, & Yarger, 2007). Selective attention deficits and behavioural disorders have been documented in a group of 7-9 yrs age preterm children, using selective spatial and visual tests and ADHD rating questionnaire for parents and teachers. Selective visual and spatial attention deficits showed to be significantly related to ADHD rating scores (Shum, Neulinger, O’Callaghan, & Mohay, 2008). The problem to differentiate ADHD and selective attentional deficits dwells in its difficulty to assess a developmental process of ADHD. Early minor motor difficulties in very preterm infants at 2 yrs corrected age have longitudinally been related to selective attention deficits at 7-9 yrs (Jeyaseelan, O’Callaghan, Neulinger, Shum, & Burns, 2006). As previously described, at this age central executive panel for working memory request more attentional resources and prolonged time employment (Saavalainen et al., 2007). Memory dysfunctions more frequently appear to be a component of the malfunctioning in other areas, especially in working memory or visual spatial memory. Functional neuro-anatomy of the hippocampus and the head of the caudate nucleus has been studied using a functional Magnetic resonance Imaging (fMRI) technique during administration of memory tasks in a group of adolescents born very preterm. Findings demonstrate no statistical difference with control group in activation of hippocampus and caudate nucleus, supporting the hypothesis that spatial memory span deficits may represent a dysfunction in executing a planned motor sequence, rather than a deficit in the ability to remember the spatial sequence. It also raises questions on the normative processes of brain maturation and potential influence of perinatal hypoxic-ischemic stress in preterm infants (Curtis, Zhuang, Townsend, Xiaoping, & Nelson, 2006). An other study supports evidence of a non-specificity of memory deficits, bringing together memory deficits with impairment in general cognitive performances (Narberhaus, Segarra, Giménez, Junqué, Pueyo, & Botet, 2007). 63 Life Span and Disability Dipasquale F. / Magnano P. 4. Conclusions The review addresses the topic of neurodevelopmental risks in preterm children. In order to analyze effective dysfunction in children born preterm without major disabilities (CP, mental retardation) previous findings of a general cognitive impairment in this population of children has not received consistent scientific agreement and has encouraged further research in this area to better assess specific neuropsychological skills and extend follow up to adolescent age. Neuroanatomical findings are consistent with developmental brain frontal lobe abnormalities and correlate with minor neurological dysfunctions which typically affect fine and gross motor development, executive function and working memory, visual-constructional and attentional performances, while memory seems to work in interconnection with other functions. Preterm birth could interfere with normal developmental brain maturational steps with functional adjustments resulting in minor dysfunctions that are more evident in preschoolers born very preterm. The large number of preterm children that tend to normalize in adolescence, with more demanding neurodevelopmental pathways than term peers, support the hypothesis of a neurodevelopmental adaptation that risks to drop into dysfunctional neuropsychological outcome. References American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and Statistic Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition, Text revision (DSM-IV-TR). APA: Washington Bayless, S., & Stevenson, J. (2007). Executive functions in school-age children born very prematurely. Early Human Development, 83, 247-254. Blair, C., Zelazo, P. D., & Greenberg, M. T. (2005). The Measurement of Executive Function in Early Childhood. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28 (2), 561-571. Bohm, B., Katz-Salamon, M., Smedler, A. C., LLagercrantz, H., & Forssberg, H. (2002). Developmental risk and protective factors for influencing cognitive outcome at 5 1/2 years of age in very-low-birth-weight children. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 44 (8), 508-16. Bohm, B., Smedler, A. C., & Forssberg, H. (2004). Impulse control, working memory and other executive functions in preterm children when starting school. Acta Paediatrica, 93, 1363-1371. Carlson, S. M. (2005). Developmentally Sensitive Measures of Executive Function in Preschool Children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28 (2), 595-616. 64 Increased risk of neuropsychological disorder in children born preterm without major disabilities Conklin, H. M., Luciana, M., Hooper, C. J., & Yarger, R. S. (2007). Working Memory Performance in typically developing children and adolescents: behavioural evidence of protracted frontal lobe development. Developmental Neuropsychology, 31 (1), 103-128. Curtis, W. J., Zhuang, J., Townsend, E. L., Xiaoping, H., & Nelson, C. A. (2006). Memory in Early Adolescents Born Prematurely: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Investigation. Developmental Neuropsychology, 29 (2), 341-377. Einspieler, C., Marschik, P. B., Milioti, S., Nakajima, Y., Bos, A. F., & Prechtl, H. F. R. (2007). Are abnormal fidgety movements an early marker for complex minor neurological dysfunction at puberty? Early Human Development, 83, 521-525. Einspieler, C., & Prechtl, H. F. R. (2005). Prechtl’s assessment of General Movements: a diagnostic tool for the functional assessment of the young nervous system. Mental Retardation And Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 11, 61- 67. Engle, W. A., Tomashek, K. M., Wallman, C., & the Committee on Fetus and Newborn (2007). “Late Preterm” infants: a population at risk”. Pediatrics, 120, 13901401. Hadders-Algra, M. (2000). The Neuronal Group Selection Theory: a framework to explain variation in normal motor development. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 42 (8), 566-572. Hadders-Algra, M. (2002). Two distinct forms of minor neurological dysfunction: perspectives emerging from a review of data of the Groningen Perinatal Project. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 44, 561-571. Hadders-Algra, M. (2003). Developmental Coordination Disorder: is Clumsy Motor Behavior caused by a Lesion of the Brain at Early age? Neural Plasticity, 1 (2), 39-50. Hadders-Algra, M. (2007). Putative neural substrate of normal and abnormal general movements. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 31 (8), 1181-90. Hadders-Algra, M., Mavinkurve-Groothuis, A. M. C., Stremmelaar, E. F., Martijn, A., & Butcher, P. R. (2004). Quality of general movements and the development of minor neurological dysfunction at toddler and school age. Clinical Rehabilitation, 18, 287-299. Hebb, D. (1949). The organization of behaviour; a neuropsychological theory. New York: Wiley-Interscience. Institute of Medicine (2006). IOM report on preterm birth. Editorial. The Lancet, 29, 368. Jeyaseelan, D., O’Callaghan, M., Neulinger, K., Shum, D., & Burns, Y. (2006). The association between early minor motor difficulties in extreme low birth weight infants and school age attentional difficulties. Early Human Development, 82, 249-255. 65 Life Span and Disability Dipasquale F. / Magnano P. Korkman, M., Mikkola, K., Ritari, N., Tommiska, V., Salokorpi, T., Haataja, L., Tammela, O., Pääkkönen, L., Olsén, P., & Fellman, V. (2008). Neurocognitive test profiles of extremely low birth weight five-year-old children differ according to neuromotor status. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33 (5), 637-55. Leijser, L. M., de Bruïne, F.T., Steggerda, S. J., van der Grond, J.,Walther, F. J., & van Wezel-Meijler, G. (2009). Brain imaging findings in very preterm infants throughout the neonatal period: Part I. Incidences and evolution of lesions, comparison between ultrasound and MRI. Early Human Development, 85, 101- 109. Leijser, L. M., Steggerda, S. J., de Bruïne, F.T., van der Grond, J.,Walther, F. J., & van Wezel-Meijler, G. (2009a). Brain imaging findings in very preterm infants throughout the neonatal period: Part II. Incidences and evolution of lesions, comparison between ultrasound and MRI. Early Human Development, 85, 111- 115. Lindström, K., Winbladh, B., Haglund, B., & Hjern, A. (2007). Preterm Infants as Young Adults: A Swedish National Cohort Study. Pediatrics. 120, 70-77. Marlow, N., Hennessy, E. M., Bracewell, M. A., & Wolke, D. (2007). Motor and Executive Function at 6 Years of Age After Extremely Preterm Birth. Pediatrics, 120, 793-804. Marlow, N., Wolke, D., Bracewell, M.A., & Samara, M. (2005). Neurologic and Developmental Disability at Six Years of Age after Extremely Preterm Birth. The New England Journal of Medicine, 352, 9-19. McQuillen, P. S., & Ferriero, D. M. (2005). Perinatal subplate neuron injury: implications for cortical development. Brain Patholology, 15, 250-260. Ment, L. R., Kesler, S., Vohr, B., Katz, K. H., Baumgartner, H., Schneider, K. C., Delancy, S., Silbereis, J., Duncan, C. C., Constable, R. T., Makuch, R. W., & Reiss, A. L. (2009). Longitudinal Brain Volume Changes in Preterm and Term Control Subjects During Late Childhood and Adolescence. Pediatrics, 123, 503-511. Molfese, D. L., Molfese, V. J., Beswick, J., Jacobi-Vessels, J., Molfese, P. J., Key, A. P. F., & Starkey, G. (2008). Dynamic links between emerging cognitive skills and brain processes. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33 (6), 682-706. Moster, D., Lie, R. T., & Markestad, T. (2008). Long-Term medical and social consequences of preterm birth. The New England Journal of Medicine, 359, 262-73. Narberhaus, A., Segarra, D., Giménez, M., Junqué, C., Pueyo, R., & Botet, F. (2007). Memory Performance in a Sample of Very Low Birth Weight Adolescents. Developmental Neuropsychology, 31 (1), 129-135. Nosarti, C., Giouroukou, E., Healy, E., Rifkin, L., Walshe, M., Reichenberg, A., Chitnis, X., Williams, S. C. R., & Murray, R. M. (2008). Grey and white matter distribution in very preterm adolescents mediates neurodevelopmental outcome. Brain, 131, 205-217. 66 Increased risk of neuropsychological disorder in children born preterm without major disabilities Petrini, J. R., Dias, T., McCormick, M. C., Massolo, M. L., Green, N. S., & Escobar. G. J. (2009). Increased risk of adverse neurological development for late preterm infants. The Journal of Pediatrics, 154, 169-76. Prechtl, H. F. R., Einspieler. C., Cioni, G., Bos, A. F., Ferrari, F., & Sontheimer, D. (1997). An early marker of developing neurological handicap after perinatal brain lesions. The Lancet, 339, 1361-1363. Pritchard, V. E., Clark, C. A. C., Liberty, K., Champion, P. R., Wilson, K., & Woodward, L. J. (2009). Early school-based learning difficulties in children born very preterm. Early Human Development, 85, 215-224. Reuner, G., Hassenpflug, A., Pietz, J., & Philippi, H. (2009). Long-term development of low-risk low birth weight preterm born infants: Neurodevelopmental aspects from childhood to late adolescence. Early Human Development, 10, 1016. Saavalainen, P., Luoma, L., Bowler, D., Määttä, S., Kiviniemi, V., Laukkanen, E., & Herrgård, E. (2007). Spatial Span in very prematurely born adolescents. Developmental Neuropsychology, 32 (3), 769-785. Saavalainen, P., Luoma, L., Laukkanen, E., Bowler, D., Määttä, S., Kiviniemi,V., & Herrgård, E. (2007a). School performance of adolescents born preterm: Neuropsychological and background correlates. European Journal of Paediatric Neurology, 12, 246-252. Salt, A., & Redshaw, M. (2006). Neurodevelopmental follow-up after preterm birth: follow up after two years. Early Human Development, 82, 185-197. Santos, A., Duret, M., Mancini, J., Gire, C., & Deruell, C. (2009). Preterm birth affects dorsal-stream functioning even after age 6. Brain and Cognition, 69, 490–494. Sastre-Riba S. (2009). Prematuridad: análisis y seguimiento de las funciones ejecutivas. Revista de Neurologìa , 48 (2), 113-118. Shum, D., Neulinger, K., O’Callaghan, M., & Mohay, H. (2008). Attentional problems in children born very preterm or with extremely low birth weight at 7-9 years. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 23, 103-112. Soria-Pastor, S., Gimenez, M., Narberhaus, A., Falcon, C., Botet, F., Bargallo, N., Mercader, J. M., & Junque, C. (2008). Patterns of cerebral white matter damage and cognitive impairment in adolescents born very preterm. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 26, 647–654. Spittle, A. J., Orton, J., Doyle, L. W., & Boyd, R. (2007). Early developmental intervention programs post hospital discharge to prevent motor and cognitive impairments in preterm infants. Cochrane Database Systematic Review, 18 (2), 5495. Stewart, A., & Kirkbride, V. (2008). Very preterm infants at fourteen years: relationship with neonatal ultrasound brain scans and neurodevelopmental status at one year. Acta Pædiatrica, 85 (416), 44-47. 67 Life Span and Disability Dipasquale F. / Magnano P. Touwen, B. C. L. (1979). The Examination of the Child with Minor Neurological Dysfunction. Clinics in Developmental Medicine n.71. London: Heinemann Medical Books. Volpe, J. J. (2009). Brain injury in premature infants: a complex amalgam of destructive and developmental disturbances. The Lancet Neurology, 8 (1), 110-124. 68 Life Span and Disability / XII, 1 (2009), 67-82 Communication Impairments in Patients with Right Hemisphere Damage Valeria Abusamra,1 Hélène Côté,2 Yves Joanette2 & Aldo Ferreres1 Abstract Right brain damages can manifest deficits of communicative skills, which sometimes cause an important inability. The communication impairments following a right hemisphere damage are distinct from those in aphasia and may affect discursive, lexico-semantic, pragmatic, and prosodic components of communication. It is calculated that this troubles affect almost a 50% of this patients. However, these impairments have essentially been studied separately and their possible coexistence in a same individual is still unknown. Moreover, the clinical profiles of communication impairments following a right hemisphere damage, including their correlation with underlying cognitive deficits, are still unreported. The goal of this article is to offer an overview of the verbal communication deficits that can be found in right-hemisphere-damaged individuals. These deficits can interfere, at different levels, with prosody, the semantic processing of words and discourse and pragmatic abilities. In spite of the incapability that they produce, communicational impairments in right brain damaged are usually neglected. Probably, the sub-diagnostic is due to the lack of an appropriate classification or to the absent of adequate assessment tools. In fact, patients with right brain damages might present harsh communicational deficits but perform correctly on aphasia tests because the last ones are not designed to detect this kind of deficit but left brain damaged impairments. Increasing our knowledge about the role of the right-hemisphere in verbal communication will have major theoretical and clinical impacts; it could * Received: 18 February 2009, Revised: 19 May 2009, Accepted: 19 May 2009. 1 Universidad de Buenos Aires and Hospital Eva Perón - Buenos Aires. Université de Montréal and Centre de Recherche, Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal, e-mail: valeriaa@fibertel.com.ar 2 69 Life Span and Disability Abusamra V. / Côté H. / Joanette Y. / Ferreri A. facilitate the diagnosis of right brain patients in the clinical circle and it will help to lay the foundations to elaborate methods and strategies of intervention. Keywords: Right hemisphere, Verbal communication, Lexical, Semantics 1. Introduction Over the last fifty years, the incorporation of pragmatics into neurolinguistics has had a strong impact on the way language impairments are conceived theoretically and clinically. Whereas late-19th-century linguistic advances emphasized the predominant role of the left hemisphere, studies during the second half of the 20th century demonstrated that rich and efficient verbal communication depends on the soundness of both cerebral hemispheres. For a long time, the clinical description of acquired language disorders (aphasias) was based on traditional linguistic components such as phonology, semantics, and morphosyntax. Thanks to contributions made by psycholinguistics and modern cognitive psychology, the very concept of language has evolved radically since the 1950s. Prosodic, discursive, and pragmatic aspects were added to the traditional dimensions of linguistics, all of which now have their own place within the study of acquired language disorders, particularly those caused by right brain hemisphere damage (RHD). It is known today that the right hemisphere plays an essential role in human behavior and that it intervenes decisively in the regulation of the pragmatic dimension of verbal communication. Although the language disorders that are present in individuals with RHD are, on the whole, different to those seen in patients with left hemisphere damage (LHD), this does not make them any less significant from a clinical point of view. A patient with RHD can, amongst other things, show difficulty transmitting communicative intentions based on emotions, on modulation of speech parameters, and on indirect meanings of discourse or figurative language. This has consequences not just for the affected individual, but also for those around them, and the functional and psychological impacts on the person’s life can be considerable. Language disorders constitute a significant obstacle to developing and maintaining satisfactory interpersonal relationships, and they can be detrimental to the resumption of an active social and professional life. Today, advances in theoretical and clinical knowledge allow us to better recognize the presence of communication disorders linked to impairment of the right cerebral hemisphere. However, affected individuals still go unnoticed by health professionals even today. Language deficits caused by 70 Communication Impairments in Patients with Right Hemisphere Damage RHD are, as a whole, different to those seen in aphasia. Despite their difficulties in processing the elemental structures of language, aphasic patients can often make use of contextual keys in order to communicate. Those with RHD have the opposite problem: although their phonological and syntactic abilities remain intact, they have serious difficulties in establishing adequate relationships between language and the context in which it is being used (Abusamra, Martínez Cuitiño, Wilson, Jaichenco, & Ferreres, 2004). As a consequence, problems caused by RHD cannot be assessed using classic aphasia evaluation batteries such as the Montreal–Toulouse Protocol for Language Assessment of Aphasia (Nespoulous, Lecours, Lafond, Lemay, Puel, Joanette et al., 1992), the Battery for the Analysis of Aphasic Deficit (Miceli, Laudanna, & Burani, 1994; Ferreres, Grus, Jacubovich, Jaichenco, Kevokian, Piaggio et al., 2000), or the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (Goodglass & Kaplan, 1979; 1983). Insofar as it is possible for these patients to present little or no deficit in formal language tests, their pragmatic impairments go unnoticed when these tools are applied. There are some specially designed protocols used to assess communication in individuals with RHD: Right Hemisphere Communication Battery, RHCB (Gardner & Brownell, 1986); Mini Inventory of Right Brain Injury, MIRBI (Pimental & Kingsbury, 1989); Right Hemisphere Language Battery, RHLB (Bryan, 1989); Ross Information Processing Assessment, RIPA (Ross-Swain, 1996); Evaluation of Communication Problems in Right Hemisphere Dysfunction, revised (Halper, Cherney, Burns, & Mogil, 1996), Protocolo de Evaluación de Funciones Lingüísticas y Comunicativas (Protocol for the Evaluation of Linguistic and Communicative Functions), PELCHD (Labos, Zabala, Atlasovich, Pavón, & Ferreiro, 2003). Despite the high quality of these protocols, most have theoretical and methodological limitations (Joannette & Ansaldo, 1999). In some cases, there has been difficulty adapting theoretical foundations to clinical demands: designs seem to be based more on practical requirements than explicit theoretical foundations. Besides, no battery takes all of the processes that have been associated with RHD into account. In 2004, Joannette, Ska & Côté introduced a protocol to be used for the evaluation of language deficits in patients with RHD into clinical settings. 2. Historical Context The systematic scientific study of the brain began during the Renaissance. However, for several centuries, the roles of the two hemispheres were not differentiated. It was not until the middle of the 19th century that the studies on the neurological impairment of language carried out by Marc Dax (1836) and Paul Broca (1865) began to demonstrate the asymmetrical 71 Life Span and Disability Abusamra V. / Côté H. / Joanette Y. / Ferreri A. nature of the brain’s functional organization. For almost a century afterwards, control of the language function was assigned exclusively to the left hemisphere, which was also considered the only cause of aphasia as a result of brain damage. As such, while the concept of cerebral dominance was asserted, the right hemisphere took on the dubious status of ‘the minor hemisphere’. With the exception of the specific roles attributed to it by a few authors (such as Jackson, 1879), the right hemisphere was sunk in a century of obscurantism and the theory of cerebral dominance denied it any role in the maintenance of linguistic behavior. In the 1960s the right hemisphere began to be assigned a role in linguistic abilities as a result of two groups of observations, one clinical and the other experimental.The first suggestions emerged from detailed clinical observation of patients with brain damage (Eisenson, 1959; 1962; Critchley, 1962; Weinstein, 1964). Nevertheless, these pioneers did not manage to create a precise, detailed description of right hemisphere function and stayed within the conceptual frameworks of the time. Eisenson (1962) talked of impairments to the “supra-ordinal” aspects of language; whereas Critchley (1962) noted the loss of “subtle” abilities, but was unable to classify them in more detail. The clinical impressions of Eisenson and Critchley are in keeping with the impairments in lexico-semantics, prosodic, discursive, and/or pragmatic skills that are described today. The second set of events that connected the right hemisphere with language abilities took place some years later. At the end of the 1960s and during the 1970s, systematic studies of the language capacities of each of the hemispheres began, amongst others, on individuals with surgical section of corpus callosum (Code, Wallesch, Joanette, & Lecours, 2002). These observations, together with others carried out with a different methodological focus, confirmed the predominance of the left cerebral hemisphere in language. At the same time, they allowed the right hemisphere’s capacities in the treatment of word meaning and other aspects of language to be identified. As more appropriate conceptual models were provided, clinical investigation led to the recognition of the language components that could be affected in patients with RHD. From then on, descriptions of the impact of right brain damage have aimed to investigate communication impairments. 3. The right hemisphere and verbal communication Right hemisphere damage can cause serious disruptions to verbal communication and can affect the prosodic, lexico-semantic, discursive, and pragmatic components of language. These components are not impaired in all patients with RHD, but approximately 50% of patients are believed to have been affected in one or more of them, which could constitute a significant disability (Joanette, Goulet, & Daoust, 1991). Thus, as brain damage 72 Communication Impairments in Patients with Right Hemisphere Damage can affect one or more of the components, different impairment profiles are generated according to which have been affected. 3.1 Prosodic impairments Prosody is the modulation of the suprasegmental parameters of speech (tone, intensity, and duration) which takes place in order to transmit a communicative, linguistic, or emotional intention. Linguistic prosody includes emphatic lexical accentuation (e.g. JOHN drinks coffee vs John drinks COFFEE) and the expression of modality or of the sentence type (e.g. statement, question). Emotional prosody refers to the variations in intonation that allow emotions to be transmitted (e.g. happiness, irritation). Numerous studies have demonstrated the presence of prosodic disorders in individuals with RHD, in terms of both perception and production (Pell, 1999; Walker & Daigle, 2000). The impairment of emotional prosody may be a very evident trait, but deficits in linguistic prosody are equally marked. In terms of production, from a clinical point of view, patients with RHD may display monotonous intonation caused by a flattening of the prosodic curve. Those with RHD tend to produce similar emotional intonation patterns to normal subjects, but with significantly reduced tone variation. This difficulty in modulating tone can affect the transmission of linguistic messages, especially those that communicate linguistic modality (Pell, 1999). In terms of perception, patients with RHD show difficulty in understanding the intention carried by their interlocutor’s prosody. Faced with an emotional prosody perception task, the identification of feelings transmitted by the interlocutor may be disturbed if the sentence has a neutral linguistic content (Tompkins & Mateer, 1985; Walker & Daigle, 2000). Although the dominant role in linguistic prosody perception is often attributed to the left hemisphere, recent studies have shown that patients with LHD perform worse than control subjects in tasks in which they have to distinguish the intonation patterns that express linguistic modality (Walker & Daigle, 2000). In short, a person with right brain damage can have serious difficulties in processing linguistic prosody and emotional prosody, both receptively and productively. 3.2 Lexico-semantic impairments The lexico-semantic dimension of language refers to the ability to understand and express words. People with RHD do not tend to exhibit marked anomia during conversation, and, on the whole, they carry out convergent tasks like naming images without difficulty, although some authors have observed minor deficits in some cases (Diggs & Basili, 1987). In contrast, subtle lexico-semantic disorders have been observed in a significant number of people with RHD when the task demands divergent semantic treatment For example, in tasks where instructions require the production of the highest possible number of words according to a given criteria (for 73 Life Span and Disability Abusamra V. / Côté H. / Joanette Y. / Ferreri A. example, “say the names of as many animals as you can” or “say as many words that begin with p as you can”), those with RHD tend to produce fewer words than control subjects and to activate peripheral semantic links, producing words that are not strongly connected and which are not very prototypical. Despite initially contradictory results, it has been shown that individuals with RHD may have difficulty when evocation must follow semantic, phonological, or orthographic criteria (Sabourin, Goulet, & Joanette, 1988) or even in the absence of any criteria, when evocation is completely free (Beausoleil, Fortín, Le Blanc, & Joanette, 2003). The capacity to establish semantic relationships between words may also be altered in patients with RHD (Chiarello & Church, 1986). This difficulty is more marked when there is a need to access words in a precise category (e.g. vegetables, tools) in order to explain the relationship (Myers & Brookshire, 1995). Lexico-semantic deficits in individuals with RHD are especially apparent when processing isolated words of low concreteness or low frequency (Joanette & Goulet, 1990). Finally, another specific difficulty that may be present in patients with RHD has to do with processing metaphoric or non-literal meanings of words (Gagnon, Goulet, Giroux, & Joanette, 2003). Patients with RHD obtain worse results than normal subjects when trying to choose from a pair of images the one that appropriately depicts a metaphorical enunciation; frequently, they opt for the one showing a literal interpretation of the enunciation (Winner & Gardner, 1977; Myers & Linebaugh, 1981). 3.3 Discursive impairments Discursive skills allow information to be transmitted by a speaker to an interlocutor in a conversational, procedural, or narrative form. Information exchange involves the expressive and receptive levels of communication, depending on whether a message is being transmitted or received. The discursive dimension has mainly been studied through narrative discourse, and it is known that this ability can be affected by RHD. As regards expression, the discourse of patients with RHD is often less informative than that of control subjects, although a similar quantity of enunciations is produced (Joanette, Goulet, Ska, & Nespoulous, 1986; Lojek-Osiejuk, 1996). Individuals with RHD provide less information than normal subjects, and this information is organized more simply, despite both groups’ narrative outputs being equal in terms of number of words and general discourse structure. Measurements of the formal aspects of lexicon and syntax show that both groups function equivalently (Jainchenco, Abusamra, & Molina, 2004). As such, the deficit must lie elsewhere: lack of coherence and a strong tendency to tangential discourse are all part of the typical profile of individuals with RHD (Davis, O´Neil-Pirozzi, & Coon, 1997). According to Wapner, Hamby and Gardner (1981), what distinguishes patients with RHD from aphasic patients is their tendency to make in74 Communication Impairments in Patients with Right Hemisphere Damage appropriate comments or to stray from the topic of the story. For example, the patient OP, who was evaluated with the MEC Protocol narrative discourse test (see Appendix, 1), produced the following discourse: There was a farmer who was digging a hole uh uh uh uh well he was digging a hole until at a certain depth...uh uh uh uh...er. who was digging a well eh eh eh so he was digging with a shovel and a pick ..uh uh uh... objects that don’t look like what we call shovel and pick I mean they have really something to do with the ground...not only uh uh uh...generally a wine...the farmer moves the it it it more with shovel than pick or at least like a pick.And so he went down to a certain depth and he was, was tired, it was night and so and the next day...he sees the well has collapsed I mean collapsed from a part of of of of You don’t remind me any more... At the receptive level, many studies show that patients with RHD have difficulty integrating the elements of a story into a coherent whole, which would allow them to make the inferences necessary to understand the text adequately. Integration difficulties become more intense when listening conditions are not ideal (Titone, Wingfield, Waters, & Prentice, 2001). In general, individuals with RHD often do not grasp – or initially grasp but subsequently lose – the main idea of a piece of discourse, and have problems suggesting a title for a story or even choosing a sentence that summarizes the main topic. This attitude is in total contrast to that of normal subjects, who willingly cast secondary elements aside in order to maintain the global coherence of the story. Problems in producing and understanding discourse are made particularly manifest in the task of retelling a story. In these cases, patients tend to introduce digressions of a personal nature as well as critiques of the story’s content. Sometimes, they may provide inadequate conclusions or produce a different story based on a specific detail of the original (tangential speech). Patients with RHD adopt a specific attitude when the story contains contradictory or unusual information. Not only are they capable of remembering the unusual elements in detail, they also have a tendency to justify them. Patients with RHD have difficulty making inferences and forming syntheses, and have trouble with some aspects of executive functions. For example, correlations have been observed between performance at tasks that require verbal inhibition and are dependent on executive functions, and text processing. Patients with RHD fail at some neuropsychological tests like the Hayling Test (Burgess & Shallice, 1997; Abusamra, Miranda, & Ferreres, 2007), a verbal completion test that measures the capacity to initiate and inhibit verbal information. The Hayling test is structured around two different sections. In the first section (initiation), the task consists of completing a sentence in a logical fashion with a word that is consistent with the context of the sentence. For example, “Juan greeted Laura with a… KISS, HUG, etc.” In the second (which measures suppression), subjects must 75 Life Span and Disability Abusamra V. / Côté H. / Joanette Y. / Ferreri A. complete each sentence with a word that is inconsistent with the context: “On the first line, write your… PILLOW, BOOMS, etc.” This difficult in inhibiting the correct option and producing a word that is far from the syntactic semantic context of the sentence correlates directly with the altered performance that patients show in text comprehension tasks. As mentioned above, in recent years a hypothesis has arisen which suggests that the performance of poor comprehenders could be due to a difficulty in inhibiting irrelevant information (Gernsbacher, 1990). Suppressing unnecessary information not only reduces interferences, it also prevents information overload on working memory. If successful comprehension depends on constructing mental representations and subsequently updating them, then selecting adequately from relevant material, focusing on the main information, and inhibiting irrelevant information would be an essential step. When this mechanism loses operational capacity – as is the case with poor comprehenders – comprehension is affected because activation is indiscriminate, and the system is overloaded and lacking in other available resources. 3.4 Pragmatic impairments Pragmatics are the linguistic and extralinguistic skills that allow an individual to process (understand and/or express) communicative intentions in a specific situational context (Gibbs, 1999). From this point of view, a speaker intends to produce a certain effect in the listener and hopes the listener will recognize it. This recognition of the speaker’s intention establishes an intentional pact, which is what makes communication possible. As such, understanding what another person is saying to us involves much more than knowing the traditional meanings of words and the relationships between them. It implies understanding the systematic relationship that is formed between what we want to say and what we actually say, between what is said and what is not, between the explicit and the implicit. As a discipline, pragmatics is at the crossroads between different fields of study: philosophy, linguistics, sociology, anthropology, and psychology. It studies how speakers understand and produce communicative acts in a concrete speech situation; that is, it is related to the use and interpretation that speakers give specific enunciations in a particular context. Pragmatic skills are especially implicated in the production and interpretation of different forms of figurative language (indirect speech acts, irony, sarcasm, humor etc.) in which the explicit or literal content of the message does not coincide with the communicative intention. One of the salient characteristics of patients with RHD is their inability to respect the principle of cooperation that regulates conversation. Amongst other things, they have difficulty respecting turn-taking, maintaining appropriate visual contact with their interlocutor, and controlling the 76 Communication Impairments in Patients with Right Hemisphere Damage progression and coherence of the topic during the communicative exchange. Some also find it complicated to adapt their verbal production to the context of the knowledge shared with each interlocutor (Chantraine, Joanette, & Ska, 1998). In this situation, patients’ contributions may seem repetitive or redundant, or, in contrast, they may consider certain information to be common knowledge even when it is not. Damage to the right hemisphere can, in some cases, cause substantial changes to an individual’s communicative behavior, affecting their capacity to adapt their messages to their interlocutor and the situational context. From a receptive point of view, some patients with RHD have difficult understanding speech acts in which the intention is not explicitly mentioned in the linguistic message. The comprehension of indirect enunciations entails going beyond their literal meaning and depends on the use of personal knowledge and an understanding of the context. The difficulty increases when the indirect enunciation is not conventional (Stemmer, Giroux, & Joanette, 1994). Indeed, patients don’t usually have difficulty interpreting conventional, established indirect speech acts like “Could you pass me the salt?”They do, however, find it hard to interpret indirect speech acts generated by specific references to the context of the conversational exchange. For example: “Oscar is moving house next Saturday. He knows it’s going to be a tough job because he has to take a lot of boxes to his new house. He runs into a friend in the street and, after telling him that he’s moving, he says, ‘What are you doing this weekend?’” Although they have already been dealt with in the section on lexico-semantic impairments, metaphors – like indirect speech acts, sarcasm, or irony – translate an intention which is different to the initial literal meaning. As such, they are also an object of study for pragmatics. What follows is an example of how the patient OP fails to interpret metaphors (see Appendix, 2). E: What does this phrase mean: My friend’s mother-in-law is a witch? P: Let’s change also one word: My son-in-law’s mother-in-law is a witch? E And so what does it mean? P.I know she is a person who hasn’t had a pleasant life, throughout her marriage.That...that she’s about to be separated from her husband; I’m referring to the mother-in-law of my son-in-law (ha, ha, ha) E. OK it’s not important- it’s the same. P. Certainly! The mother-in-law of my son-in-law. The mother-in-law of my son-in-law is a witch! E What does being a witch mean? P Because the woman is separated, because all her life she has criticized her husband for the way he is; only seen in his defects, who has kept his daughter all her life under a glass bell and she’s now a poor lady because she can’t find the fiancè her mother would like. 77 Life Span and Disability Abusamra V. / Côté H. / Joanette Y. / Ferreri A. E So what does witch mean, then? P What does it specifically mean? It means being tied-down to religious sects, to religions, to umbanda… who knows, there are so many. E So therefore, “The mother-in-law of my son-in-law is a witch”? Does it mean the mother-in-law of my friend practices black magic? And the mother-in-law of my friend has many brooms and she is also a bad person an rude? P: It’s absolutely clear. My friend’s mother-in-law has many brooms...no! My friend’s mother-in-law practices black magic. The patient is unable to understand the metaphor not only because he interprets each option literally, but also because he cannot avoid referring to himself with each metaphor he is presented with. This behavior, strongly linked to the literal, is one of the peculiarities of patients with RHD. 4. Communication impairment profiles Very few studies have dealt with the occurrence rate of communication impairments and the possible clinical profiles in patients with RHD. However, clinical experience clearly demonstrates that not all patients with RHD have trouble with the communication deficits described thus far. According to Joanette et al. (1991), approximately 50% of patients may be affected by one or more communicative impairments. This proportion is similar to that of people with LHD who suffer persistent language disorders (aphasias). When present, deficits seem to be the result of cortical damage, generally in the perisylvian area, as is the case with aphasia. However, a recent study on a random group of patients with RHD found that around 80% of the sample showed communication impairments when diagnosed using structured clinical observation as well as formal evaluation (Côté, Payer, Giroux, & Joanette, 2007). The way in which communication deficits present themselves clinically is also very significant. As happens with aphasias, not all patients with RHD and communicative disorders have the same deficit pattern. In some cases, only one of the components (prosody, semantic processing, discourse, or pragmatics) is affected; in others, the impairment can be detected in more than one component. As such, the clinical patterns of communication impairments are heterogeneous. The work of Côté et al. (2007) focused on this issue by applying a hierarchical cluster analysis to the results of 28 patients with RHD using MEC protocol. The study identified four groups on the basis of the similarity of patient results in protocol tasks that evaluated communication skills. In the first group, impairments were observed in the four areas the protocol evaluates: lexico-semantic, discursive, prosodic, and pragmatic skills. The second group was characterized by having retained its 78 Communication Impairments in Patients with Right Hemisphere Damage discursive skills, relatively speaking. None of the skills of the third group were affected, and the fourth only showed a deficit in their lexico-semantic skills. This same study found no correlation between the location of the RHD and the pattern of impairment to communicative skills. However, the third group (which obtained normal results) showed a high incidence of subcortical damage. On the other hand, the majority of patients with frontal damage were in group two (characterized by the relative retention of discursive skills). In short, communication impairments can be observed in at least half of all patients with RHD. When these impairments manifest themselves, they can take a variety of clinical forms, from an isolated disorder in one component of communication to various clinical profiles associated with impairments in more than one component. For this reason, physicians must be especially careful in their diagnosis, and should not expect all patients with RHD to behave identically. 5. Conclusions The association between the left hemisphere in right-handed people and language ability has recently been explored in more detail. The right hemisphere in right-handed people has also proved to contain some skills for processing certain components of language, more related to content than to form. Along these lines, it has been proved that RHD can cause impairments to prosody, the semantic processing of words, and discursive and pragmatic skills. Although the exact origins of this disorder remain unknown, it is thought that they may correspond to specific deficits in patients with RHD, to impairments that can be present both in individuals with RHD and LHD, or even to a non-specific impact on the limited availability of cognitive resources. Although the rate of occurrence of communication disorders in patients with RHD is yet to be determined, no estimates place it below 50%. In clinical practice, these patients tend to be under-diagnosed, which is due as much to a lack of clinical suspicion as to the fact that the batteries which evaluate aphasia do not detect their impairments. The specific evaluation tools that are now available allow non-select populations of patients with RHD to be studied. This will allow the occurrence rate of communication impairments to be reliably determined, which will, in turn, have an impact on the development of treatment techniques and the adaptation of relevant health policies. 79 Life Span and Disability Abusamra V. / Côté H. / Joanette Y. / Ferreri A. Bibliografía Abusamra, V., Miranda, A. Y., & Ferreres, A. (2007). Evaluación de la iniciación e inhibición verbal en español. Adaptación y normas del test de Hayling. Revista Argentina de Neuropsicología, 9, 19-32. Abusamra, V., Martínez Cuitiño, M., Wilson, M., Jaichenco, V., & Ferreres, A. (2004). Perspectiva pragmática en la evaluación de los lesionados derechos. Buenos Aires: Universidad de Buenos Aires, Facultad de Psicología. Beausoleil, N., Fortín, R., Le Blanc, B., & Joanette, Y. (2003). Unconstrained oral naming performance in right and left-hemisphere-damaged individuals: when education overrides the lesion. Aphasiology, 17, 143-158. Bryan, K. L. (1989). The right hemisphere language battery. (2nd ed.). Kibworth, England: Far communications. Broca, P. (1865). Sur la faculté du langage articulé. Bulletin de la Société d´Anthropologie, 6, 337-393. Burgess, P. W., & Shallice, T. (1997). The Hayling and Brixton Tests. Thurston, Suffolk: Thames Valley Test Company. Chantraine, Y., Joanette, Y., & Ska, B. (1998). Conversational abilities in patients with right hemisphere damage. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 11 (1-2), 21-32. Chiarello, C., & Church, K. L. (1986). Lexical judgement after right or left hemisphere injury. Neuropsychologia, 24, 623-640. Code, C., Wallesch, C. W., Joanette, Y., & Lecours, A. R. (2002). Classic cases in neuropsychology, vol II. Have, England: Psychology Press. Côté, H., Payer, M., Giroux, F., & Joanette, Y. (2007). Towars a description of clinical communication impairments profiles following right-hemisphere damages. Aphasiology, 21, 739-749 Critchley, M. (1962). Speech and speech loss in relation to duality of the brain. In V. B. Mountcastle (Ed.), Interhemispheric relations and cerebral dominance (pp. 208213). Baltimore, MD: John Hopkins Press. Dax, M. (1836; 1865). Lésions de la moitié gauche de l ´encéphale coincident avec l´oubli des signes de la pensée (Lu au Congrès méridional tenu à Montpellier en 1835). Gazette Hebdomadaire de Médicine et de Chirugie, 2ème série, 2, 259-262. Davis, G. A., O´Neil-Pirozzi, T. M., & Coon, M. (1997). Referential cohesion and logical coherence of narration after right hemisphere stroke. Brain and Language, 56 (2), 183-210. Diggs, C. C., & Basili, A. G (1987). Verbal expression of right cerebrovascular acci80 Communication Impairments in Patients with Right Hemisphere Damage dent patients: Convergent and divergent language. Brain and Language, 30, 130146. Eisenson, J. (1959). Language dysfunctions associated with right brain damage. American Speech and Hearing Association, 1, 107. Eisenson, J. (1962). Language and intellectual modifications associated with right cerebral damage. Language Speech, 5, 49-53. Ferreres, A., Grus, J., Jacubovich, S., Jaichenco, V., Kevokian, A., Piaggio, V., Politis, D., & Recio, F. (2000). Batería para el Análisis de los Déficits Afásicos. Buenos Aires: JVE Ediciones. Ferreres, A., Abusamra, V., Cuitiño, M, Côté, H., Ska, B., & Joanette, Y. (2007). Protocolo MEC. Protocolo para la Evaluación de la Comunicación de Montreal. Buenos Aires: Neuropsi Ediciones. Gagnon, L., Goulet, P., Giroux, F., & Joanette, Y. (2003). Processing of metaphoric and non-metaphoric alternative meanings of words after right and left hemisphere lesion. Brian and Language, 87, 217- 226. Gardner, H., & Brownell, H. H. (1986). Right Hemisphere Communication Battery. Boston: Psychology Service, VAMC. Gernsbacher, M. A. (1990). Language comprehension as structure building. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Gibbs, R. (1999). Interpreting what speakers say and implicate. Brain and Language, 68, 466-485. Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1979). Evaluación de la Afasia y de Trastornos Similares. Buenos Aires: Editorial Médica Panamericana. Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1983). The assessment of aphasia and related disorders. Philadelphia: Lea and Febiger. Halper,A. S., Cherney, L. R., Burns, M. S., & Mogil, S. I. (1996). RIC Evaluation of communication problems in right hemisphere dysfunction-revised. Rockville, MD: Aspen. Jackson, J. H. (1879). On affections of speech form disease of the brain. Brain, 2, 203-222. Joanette, Y., & Goulet, P. (1990). Narrative discourse in right brain damage right handers. In H. H Brownel & Y. Joanette (Eds). Discourse ability and brain damage: Theoretical and empirical perspectives (pp 131-153). New York: Springer-Verlag. Jainchenco, V., Abusamra, V., & Molina, G. (2004). Cuadernos de neurolingüística 4: hemisferio derecho y comunicación verbal. Buenos Aires: OPFYL. Universidad de Buenos Aires. Facultad de Filosofía y Letras. 81 Life Span and Disability Abusamra V. / Côté H. / Joanette Y. / Ferreri A. Joanette, Y., Goulet, P., & Daoust, H. (1991). Incidence et profils des troubles de la communication verbale chez les cérébrolésés droits. Revue de Neuropsychologie, 1 (1), 3-27. Joanette, Y., Goulet, P., Ska, B., & Nespoulous, J. L. (1986). Informative content of narrative discourse in right-brain-damaged right-handers. Brain and Language, 29, 81-105. Joanette, Y., & Ansaldo, A. I. (1999). Clinical note: Acquired pragmatic impairments and aphasia. Brain and Language, 68, 529-534. Joanette, Y., Ska, B., & Côté, H. (2004). Protocole Montréal d´évaluation de la communication (MEC). Isbergues, France: Ortho-Edition. Labos, E., Zabala, K., Atlasovich, C., Pavón, H., & Ferreiro, J. (2003). Evaluación de funciones lingüísticas y comunicativas en pacientes con lesión del hemisferio derecho. Revista Neurológica Argentina, 28, 96-103. Lojek-Osiejuk, E. (1996). Knowledge of scripts reflected in discourse of aphasics and right-brain-damaged patients. Brain and Language, 53, 58-80. Miceli, G., Laudanna, A., & Burani, C. (1994) Batería para el análisis de los déficits afásicos (BADA) (Traducción de Ferreres, A., Grus, J., Jacubovich, S., Jaichenco, V., Kevokian, A., Piaggio, V., Politis, D., & Recio, F.). Buenos Aires: JVE. Myers, P. S., & Linegaugh, C. W. (1981). Comprehension of idiomatic expression by right-hemisphere-damaged adults. In R.H. Brookshire (Ed). Clinical Aphasiology: Conference Proceedings. Minneapolis, MN: BRK Publishers. Myers, P. S., & Brookshire, R. H. (1995). Effect of noun type on naming performance of right-hemisphere-damaged and non-brain-damaged adults. Clinical Aphasiology, 23, 195-206. Nespoulous, J. L., Lecours, A. R., Lafond, D., Lemay, A., Puel, M., Joanette, Y., Cot, F., & Rascol, A. (1992). Protocole Montréal-Toulouse d´examen linguistique de l´aphasie (MT-86). Isbergues, France: Ortho Edition. Pell, M. D. (1999). Fundamental frequency encoding of linguistic and emotional prosody by right- hemisphere- damaged speakers. Brain and Language, 69, 2, 161-192. Pimental, P. A., & Kingsbury, N. A. (1989). Mini inventory of right brain injury (2nd. ed.). Austin, Texas: PRO-ED. Ross-Swain, D. G. (1996). Ross information processing assessment (2nd. ed.) Texas: PRO-ED. Sabourin, L., Goulet, P., & Joanette, Y. (1988). Word naming in right-brian-damaged right handers: Effect of level of productivity of criteria. Poster presented at the 26th annual meeting of the Academy of aphasia, Montréal. 82 Communication Impairments in Patients with Right Hemisphere Damage Stemmer, B., Giroux, F., & Joanette, Y. (1994). Production and evaluation of requests by right hemisphere brain-damaged individuals. Brain and Language, 47, 131. Titone, D., Wingfield, A., Waters, G., & Prentice, K. (2001). Memory and encoding of spoken discourse following right hemisphere damage. Evidence form the Auditory Moving Window (AMW) technique. Brain and Language, 77, 1, 10-24. Tompkins, C. A., & Mateer, C. A (1985). Right hemisphere appreciation of prosodic and linguistic indications of implicit attitude. Brain and Language, 24, 185-203. Walker, J. P., & Daigle, T. (2000). Hemispheric specialization in processing prosodic structures: Revisited. Brain and Language, 36, 580-591. Wapner, W., Hamby, S., & Gardner, H. (1981). The role of the right hemisphere in the apprehension of complex linguistic materials. Brain and Language, 14, 15-33. Winner, E., & Gardner, H. (1977). The comprehension of metaphor in brain-damaged patients. Brain, 100, 719-727. Weinstein, E. A. (1964). Affection of speech with lesions of the non-dominant hemisphere. Research Publications of the Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease, 42, 220-228. Enclosed 1. Narrative text - MEC Protocol (Joanette et al., 2004; Ferreres et al., 2007) John is a farmer from the north. He has been busy for several days digging a well on his land. The work is almost over. This morning John has arrived to finish his work and sees that during the night the well has collapsed and half of it is filled with earth. He’s very upset about this. He thinks for some minutes and says to himself, “I have an idea.” He leaves his shirt and cap on the edge of the well, hides the pick and pail, and climbs up a tree to hide himself. Later, a neighbor passes by and approaches John to talk to him a little. When he sees his shirt and cap he thinks John is working at the bottom of the well. The fellow passes near by, bends down a little, and sees the well half-filled with dirt and starts to desperately cry out, “Help! Help! Friends! Come immediately! John is buried under the well.!...”The neighbors run towards the well and start digging to save poor John.When the neighbors stop taking away the earth, John comes down the tree, approaches them and says, “Thanks a lot, you’ve been a great help.” 83 Life Span and Disability Abusamra V. / Côté H. / Joanette Y. / Ferreri A. 2. Examples of metaphors - MEC Protocol (Joanette et al., 2004; Ferreres et al., 2007) 1) The Chemistry Professor is a sweet A. The Chemistry professor is made of chocolate B. The Chemistry professor is very desirable and very attractive C. The Chemistry Professor produces sweets 2) Is my friend’s mother-in-law a witch? A. My friend’s mother-in-law practices black magic B. My friend’s mother-in-law has many brooms C. My friend’s mother-in-law is very mean and rude. 84 Life Span and Disability / XII, 1 (2009), 83-110 International Literature Review on WHODAS II (World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule II) Stefano Federici,1 Fabio Meloni,2 & Alessandra Lo Presti1 Abstract This review is a critical analysis regarding the study and utilization of the World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule II (WHODAS II) as a basis for establishing specific criteria for evaluating relevant international scientific literature. The WHODAS II is an instrument developed by the World Health Organisation in order to assess behavioural limitations and restrictions related to an individual’s participation, independent from a medical diagnosis. This instrument was developed by the WHO’s Assessment, Classification and Epidemiology Group within the framework of the WHO/NIH Joint Project on Assessment and Classification of Disablements. To ascertain the international dissemination level of for WHODAS II’s utilization and, at the same time, analyse the studies regarding the psychometric validation of the WHODAS II translation and adaptation in other languages and geographical contests. Particularly, our goal is to highlight which psychometric features have been investigated, focusing on the factorial structure, the reliability, and the validity of this instrument. International literature was researched through the main data bases of indexed scientific production: the Cambridge Scientific Abstracts – CSA, PubMed, and Google Scholar, from 1990 through to December 2008. The following search terms were used: “whodas”, in the field query, plus “title” and “abstract”. * Received: 19 March 2009, Revised: 8 May 2009, Accepted: 8 May 2009. 1 Department of Human and Educational Sciences, University of Perugia, Interdisciplinary Research Centre on Disability and Technologies for Autonomy (CIRID) “Sapienza” University of Rome, e-mail: [email protected] 2 Ph.D. in Cognitive, Psycho-physiological, and Personality Psychology, Interuniversity Center for Research on Cognitive Processing in Natural and Artificial System (ECONA) “Sapienza” University of Rome. 3 Interdisciplinary Research Centre on Disability and Technologies for Autonomy (CIRID) “Sapienza” University of Rome. 85 Life Span and Disability Federici S. / Meloni F. / Lo Presti A. The WHODAS II has been used in 54 studies, of which 51 articles are published in international journals, 2 conference abstracts, and one dissertation abstract. Nevertheless, only 7 articles are published in journals and conference proceedings regarding disability and rehabilitation. Others have been published in medical and psychiatric journals, with the aim of indentifying comorbidity correlations in clinical diagnosis concerning patients with mental illness. Just 8 out of 51 articles have studied the psychometric properties of the WHODAS II. The instruments have been translated into 11 languages and administered to a total of 88,844 subjects. Finally, the WHODAS II is prevalently used in the medical field, with major emphasis in the specialities of psychiatry, general medicine, and rehabilitation. All studies point out that WHODAS II as an effective and reliable instrument in order to assess the disability, individual functioning and participation levels. Furthermore, they often suggest administering the WHODAS II along with quality of life measures. Finally, the studies about the psychometric properties of the instrument agree in considering the WHODAS II a reliable and valid tool for disability assessment. Keywords: WHODAS II, WHO classifications, Biopsychosocial model, Disability classifications 1. Introduction 1.1. The classifications of disability: ICIDH and ICF 1.1.1. The ICIDH Since 1948 the World Health Organization (WHO) has been the specialized agency of the United Nations to review the international nomenclature of diseases and standardize the methods of diagnosis (WHO, 1948). The success obtained from the edition of International Classification of Disease (ICD) led, in the early 1970s, to the preparation of a classification of the consequences of disease. Since 1975 there has been in circulation, as an internal document of the WHO, a version of the International Classification of Impairments, Disabilities, and Handicaps (ICIDH). Subsequently, the WHO requested Philip Wood to collect the material produced until then and transform it into a classification. In 1980 the WHO published the results, the ICIDH, in a book for study and research (World Health Organization (WHO), 1980; cfr. also: Pfeiffer, 1998; Üstün, Bickenbach, Badley, & Chatterji, 1998). The aim of the ICIDH was to clarify some concepts and terminology that were used with reference to disability, to facilitate research and policy choices in an area of growing importance. The classification has been translated into many languages and used to conduct statistical surveys on population, to encode information on the health of people 86 International Literature Rewiew on WHODAS II and as a starting point for the implementation of social and welfare policies. The ICIDH has an unquestionable merit: it introduced, from the health point of view, a first-time distinction and definition of terms that, until then, had been used interchangeably, creating considerable confusion among health professionals. The ICIDH proposes a tripartite distinction between Impairment, Disability and Handicap, defined as follows: - Impairment: any loss, or abnormality, of psychological, physiological or anatomical structures or functions. - Disability: any limitation or loss (due to an impairment) of ability to perform an activity or variations in the way considered normal for a human being. - Handicap: disadvantage experienced by a particular person, the result of an impairment or a disability that limits or prevents the opportunity to fill the role usually just one person (in relation to age, sex and socio-cultural factors). In the definition of handicap a clear causal relationship is established between handicap and other conditions, i.e. the handicap is always the result of an impairment or the consequence of a disability. Therefore, the impairment, or disability, or both, are necessary so that we can talk about handicap; and yet, they are not sufficient, since not all impairments produce handicap. It is essential, according to the ICIDH, that the handicap is lived or experienced as such, that the person is aware of the disproportion between expected performance and that actually given because of the condition of disability. The ICIDH was designed with the intent to offer a non-medical model of disability, and this is demonstrated by the substantial lack of aetiological factors. And yet, as the ICIDH declares that among the three levels of impairment, disability and handicap there is a relationship that can not be simply linear, literature evaluates the classification as the product of a cultural context in which the handicap was considered the product of an impairment and/or a disability. While it is acknowledged that the ICIDH is undoubtedly a tool developed with the goal of utilizing a common and universal language on disability at an international level (Üstün, Bickenbach, et al., 1998; Bickenbach, Chatterji, Badley, & Üstün, 1999; Buono & Zagaria, 1999; Üstün, Chatterji, et al., 2001), it has been the focus of great controversy, especially animated by the supporters of the social model of disability who considered the Classification too oriented towards the medical model (Chamie, 1995; Pfeiffer, 1998), despite what is claimed by its editors (Bury, 2000). In any case, we can only note that each of the three key concepts of classification is defined in relation to a concept of normality that it is assumed to be related primarily to biomedical categories. 87 Life Span and Disability Federici S. / Meloni F. / Lo Presti A. 1.1.2. The ICF In 2001, the World Health Organization adopted the new International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health. The final document collects work published over the last decade and which has had as its goal revision of the ICIDH. The nine years dedicated to completing the review process will certainly give an idea of the complexity of the problems dealt with and the extent of the criticisms raised by the proposal for a new Classification (Üstün, Bickenbach, et al., 1998; Pfeiffer, 1998; Hurst, 2000; Pfeiffer, 2000). As has already been pointed-out, the criticisms about several conceptual aspects of the ICIDH, which has determined the need for a revision, are: - The reference to a medical model of disability, which is sequential and causal, according to which disability (or/and handicap) is regarded as the direct outcome of an impairment of the individual. - The application of an approach based on a linear succession considering the handicap as a direct consequence of impairment. - The presence of a negative terminological bias, as most conditions are described by using a negative terminology. From an operative viewpoint, the main limitations characterising the ICIDH were given by the use of terms which were inadequate with reference to the contemporary scientific context, as well as by the impossibility to compare data from different contexts (Chatterji et al., 2001; Rehm et al., 2001; Trotter et al., 2001; Üstün, Chatterji et al., 2001). The linear progressive perspective applied in the old classification is abandoned in the ICF, to implement a circular interactive model in which functioning and disability of a person are considered as the product of the dynamic interaction between health conditions and contextual factors, including personal and environmental ones. The structure of this new classification can thus be divided into two “parts”, each one including two “components”: Part 1, “Functioning and Disability”; including the following components: a) body functions and structures and b) activities and participation; Part 2, “Contextual factors”, including the following components: a) environmental factors and b) personal factors. Each component is formed by several domains, and each domain is organised in categories at different levels, which represents the units of classification. Moreover, in contrast with the ICIDH, the ICF sets a common, “standard” language, which not only allows a common understanding and use by operators belonging to different professional areas, but is also easily applicable to remarkably different environmental contexts. There are two consequences stemming from this approach: - First, the context and the life environment of each individual dramatically influences the level of her/his functioning in presence of a given disability and, given the same impairment, different contexts have very diverse effects on individual functioning and adaptation. 88 International Literature Rewiew on WHODAS II - Secondly, any person during her/his life can experience a changing state of health which, in a given environment, becomes disabling, i.e. influencing negatively on the person’s functioning abilities. The ICF, wanting to describe functional states of each individual and his/her limitations, proposes a dynamic model of mutual interaction between health conditions and contextual factors. The presence of an impairment necessarily implies a “cause”, which may not be sufficient to explain the result of impairment. Therefore, the disability is the complex and multiderminated outcome of three main factors: the health of an individual, the personal and environmental factors. The triadic reciprocal causation of factors exceeds the linear etiological prospect which from altered states of health leads to disability. In the new biopsychosocial model, the disability, understood both as a limitation of individual abilities as well as restrictions in social participation, is certainly related to a state of health, conventionally regarded as pathological, but not necessarily caused by the same condition as in the linear model of the ICIDH. The biopsychosocial model provides a perspective on the health concept that is not always in line with the medical one. Since different environments may have a very different impact on the same individual with a certain health condition, like the ICF notes «two persons with the same disease may have different levels of functioning and two persons with the same level of functioning not necessarily have the same condition of health» (ICF, p. 12). The interconnections between biological, structural, functional factors, of abilities, social participation, various contexts and personal and psychological dimensions do not allow simple aetiologies, focusing only on the physiopathological, anatomical and neurological levels. 1.2. Traditional tools for measuring and assessing the disabilit. Specific rating scales for measuring disability can be regarded as the Barthel Index and FIM (Functional Independence Measure). The first one has the advantage that it can be administered quickly and without special training; the second one involves slightly longer times of administration and requires specific training. The Barthel Index (Mahoney & Barthel, 1965) is an ordinal scale with total score from 0 (totally dependent) to 100 (totally independent) and comprising 10 items. The index shows the level of autonomy in various activities: feeding, taking a bath, personal hygiene, dressing, rectum and bladder control, transfers to bathroom or chair/bed, walking and climbing stairs. The performance should be established using the best available data, the usual sources are direct questions to the patient, friends/relatives and nurses, but also direct observation and common sense are important. Excellent validity and reliability are the strong points of the index that, however, appears to be subject to a “plateau” effect in highlighting the changes in more 89 Life Span and Disability Federici S. / Meloni F. / Lo Presti A. complex functions. Reflecting a background determined by the cultural prevalence of the medical model, the Barthel index assigns an absolutely relevant weight to functions such as continence or mobility and not the least, also explores self-sufficiency in cognitive areas. Moreover, it is not a real standard, since there are at least 8 different versions published that differ in the number of items and methodology in assignment of scores. Also the FIM (Keith, Granger, Hamilton, & Sherwin, 1987) measures self-sufficiency in 18 activities of daily living (like dressing, feeding, locomotion, etc.) that cumulatively provide a quantitative index of disability. Beyond the advantages of scale, such as the statistical validity, the simplicity of implementation and the ability to compare data at the international level, thanks to its wide distribution, the FIM is an instrument that assesses the level of self-sufficiency of a person from the perspective of an outside observer, leaving no space for self-evaluation. 1.3. The assessment of disability according to the biopsychosocial model The direct application of the ICF and its codes appeared since the beginning as a rather demanding and complex task: for this reason, the WHO introduced the ICF Checklist (WHO, 2003), which allows the description of the functioning profile of a subject based on 128 codes selected among the thousands forming the whole ICF (in the second level there are already 362 codes, that become 1.424 in the third and fourth level) (ivi, p. 3). The ICF checklist is not really an instrument for measure or assessment: its utility comes from the possibility to “open” the codes on the basis of the identification of a person’s functioning problem, and at the same time to establish whether, and in which measure, the environment acts either as barrier or conversely facilitates the individual. The ICF Checklist is administered to the patient or his/her caregiver. It is structurally divided into four parts: the introductory part, which includes biographical data, the ICD-10 code, and the specification of information source; the first part, containing the list of codes of Body Functions (b) and Body Structures (s); the second part, comprising the list of codes for Activities and Participation (d); and finally, the third part, containing the list of codes relating to Environmental Factors (e). In Italy, the translation, validation, and a first application in the research and clinical field were coordinated by the Disability Italian Network (DIN) in 2004. The WHODAS II, however, proposes to evaluate the disability from a different viewpoint from that of the normal tools of measurement. In fact, while the ICF Checklist was developed as a practical tool to elicit clinicians’ overall impressions of a patient’s condition and to record information on functioning and disability, the WHODAS II rates the nature of disability directly from the patient’s responses. Therefore, the ICF Checklist offers an external (objective) view on disability while the WHODAS II does an internal (subjective) one. 90 International Literature Rewiew on WHODAS II The WHODAS II assesses the limitations in activities and restrictions in participation experienced by an individual, independently from a medical diagnosis. Specifically, the instrument is designed to evaluate the functioning of the individual in six activity domains: 1. Understanding and communicating 2. Getting around 3. Self-care 4. Getting along with people 5. Life activities 6. Participation in society There are different forms of the WHODAS II, each of them has been structured in relation to the number of item (6, 12, 24, 12 + 24 and 36), the mode of administration (self-administered or administered by an interviewer) and the user to whom the interview is proposed (subject, clinician, caregiver). In any case, the WHO recommends the use of the 36 item form administered by an interviewer for completeness. The participants interviewed are asked to indicate the experienced level of “difficulty” (none, mild, moderate, severe, extreme), by taking into account the way in which they normally perform a given activity, and including the use of whatever support or/and help by a person (aids). For every item receiving a positive answer, the subsequent question asks the number of days (“in the last 30 days”) in which the interviewed has met such a difficulty, in terms of a 5-point ordinal scale: 1) Only one day; 2) Up to a week = from 2 to 7 days; 3) Up to two weeks = from 8 to 14 days; 4) More than two weeks = from 15 to 29 days; 5) Every day = 30 days. Then, the person is asked how much the difficulties have interfered with his/her life. Respondents should answer the questions according to the following references: 1. Degree of difficulty (the increase in the effort, discomfort or pain, or slowness, or differences in general); 2. Health conditions (disease or illness, or injury, or mental or emotional problems, or related to alcohol, or problems associated with drug abuse); 3. The last 30 days; 4. The average between “good” and “bad” days; 5. The way in which they normally perform the activity. The items that refer to activities not experienced in the last 30 days are not classified. 2. Purpose and methodology The general aim of the study presented here is to check the spread of the WHODAS II at international level and in different fields of application 91 Life Span and Disability Federici S. / Meloni F. / Lo Presti A. Specifically, given the widespread consent universally reached about the usefulness of the WHODAS II, we need to verify its reliability in assessing the functioning and the self-perception of disability in persons with normal abilities and disabled participants, both through the analysis of some psychometric characteristics such as reliability, validity and factorial structure, either through correlational analysis. The bibliographic review, in the next paragraph, is intended to provide an overview, as complete as possible, of scientific studies that have been made using the WHODAS II, since its publication until now. In most of these studies, moreover, the WHODAS II was used in combination with other assessment tools: this has allowed us to verify its convergent validity, and its compatibility and complementarities with these instruments. A survey on the main databases of international indexed scientific production, Cambridge Scientific Abstracts – CSA and PubMed, using as key search the term “whodas” in the “title” and “abstract” field query, it was found that the WHODAS II was used in 54 works. Table 1 shows the list of the 54 studies, specifying for each the type of study, the number of participants, the nationality, the field of research and the main purposes and results. 92 United States Italy India Canada Quantitative empirical study of clinical treatment Qualitative empirical study Correlational quantitative empirical study Correlational quantitative empirical study Psychometric quantitative empirical study 3. Badr et al. (2007). Role of Gender in Coping Capabilities among Young Visually Disabled Students. 4. Banerjee et al. (2008). Prevalence of depression and its effect on disability in patients with agerelated macular degeneration. 5. Baron et al. (2008). The clinimetric properties of the world health organization disability assessment schedule II in early inflammatory arthritis. 6. Bonnewyn et al. (2005). The impact of mental disorders on daily functioning in the Belgian community. 7. Buist-Bouwman et al. (2008). Psychometric properties of the World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule used in the European Study of the Epidemiology of Mental Disorders. 8. Chisolm et al. (2005). The WHODAS II: psychometric properties in the measurement of functional health status in adults with acquired hearing loss. Psychometric quantitative empirical study United States Epidemiological Belgium correlational quantitative empirical study Psychometric Netherlands quantitative empirical study Egypt Nationality Type of study Articles published in international journals 1. Alexopoulos et al. (2003). Problem-solving therapy versus supportive therapy in geriatric major depression with executive dysfunction. 2. Annicchiarico et al. (2004). Qualitative profiles of disability. Tab. 1 - International literature on WHODAS II Disability and rehabilitatio n Psychiatry 8796 380 Medicine Medicine Disability and rehabilitatio n Disability and rehabilitatio n Psychiatry Area of research Psychiatry 2419 172 53 200 96 25 Subjects Definition of the psychometric properties of the WHODAS II for a sample of adults with onset of hearing loss. Validation of the version of WHODAS used in the European Study of the Epidemiology of Mental Disorders (ESEMeD). Assessing the impact of mental disorders on daily functioning of the Belgian population. Evaluation of clinimetric properties of the WHODAS II in patients with early inflammatory arthritis. Assessment of depression effects on disability in patients with visual macular degeneration. Evaluation of the role of gender in coping skills among young visually disabled students. Identification of profiles of functional disability parallel to increased levels of disability. Comparison of the effectiveness of problem-solving therapy and supportive care in a group of elderly subjects with executive dysfunction. Purposes Good reliability and validity. Good reliability and validity and factorial structure confirmed. Correlation occurred. Good reliability and validity. Correlation occurred. Correlation occurred. Identification of four groups of individuals with disabilities. Effectiveness of treatment for problemsolving recognized. Results International Literature Rewiew on WHODAS II 93 94 Belgium, Germany, Italy, Spain, France and Netherlands Italy Ireland Epidemiological correlational quantitative empirical study Psychometric quantitative empirical study Correlational quantitative empirical study 14. ESEMeD/MHEDEA 2000 investigators. (2004). Disability and quality of life impact of mental disorders in Europe. 15. Federici et al. (2008). World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule II (WHODAS II): A contribution to the Italian validation. 16. Gallagher et al. (2004). Levels of ability and functioning: using the WHODAS II in an Irish context. Turkey Correlational quantitative empirical study Turkey United States Psychometric quantitative empirical study Correlational quantitative empirical study Australia Australia Correlational quantitative empirical study Psychometric quantitative empirical study 13. Ertugrul et al. (2004). Perception of stigma among patients with schizophrenia. 10. Chopra et al. (2008). Comparison of disability and quality of life measures in patients with long-term psychotic disorders and patients with multiple sclerosis: an application of the WHO Disability Assessment Schedule II and WHO Quality of Life-BREF. 11. Chwastiak et al. (2003). Disability in depression and back pain: evaluation of the World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule (WHO DAS II) in a primary care setting. 12. Donmez et al. (2005). Disability and its effects on quality of life among older people living in Antalya city center, Turkey. 9. Chopra et al. (2004). The assessment of patients with longterm psychotic disorders: Application of the WHO Disability Assessment Schedule II. 1304 500 21425 60 840 149 40 20 Disability and rehabilitatio Disability and rehabilitatio n Psychiatry Psychiatry Medicine Medicine Psychiatry Psychiatry Correlational analysis between sociodemographic variables, causes of disability and domains of individual f i i d bili di db Detection of frequency and severity level of disability for older people living in Antalya city center; evaluation of the effects of disability and variables associated with it on living conditions. Measurement of the relationship between the symptoms and other characteristics of schizophrenic patients with self-perceived stigma. Survey on the impact of the state of mental health and specific mental and physical disorders on work performance and quality of life in six European countries. Validation of the Italian version of the WHODAS II. Evaluation of measurement properties of the WHODAS II in two disorders commonly encountered in primary care setting. Comparison between the application of the WHODAS II and the WHOQOLBREF in the evaluation of patients with psychotic disorders and multiple sclerosis. Evaluation of the WHODAS II in patients treated for long-term psychotic disorders. Correlations confirmed. Good validity and reliability and factorial structure confirmed. Correlations occurred. Correlation occurred. Frequency and severity detected; correlation detected. Good validity and responsiveness to change. Correlation confirmed. Good reliability and validity. Life Span and Disability Federici S. / Meloni F. / Lo Presti A. Psychometric quantitative empirical study 19. Hudson et al. (2008). Quality of life in systemic sclerosis: psychometric properties of the World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule II. 20. Janca et al. (1996). The World Health Organization Short Disability Assessment Schedule (WHO DAS-S): a tool for the assessment of difficulties in selected areas of functioning of patients with mental disorders. 21. Kemmler et al. (2003). Quality of life of HIV-infected patients: Psychometric properties and validation of the German version of the MQOL-HIV. 95 24. Kim et al. (2008). BDNF genotype potentially modifying the 22. Kessler et al. (2003). The Epidemiology of Major Depressive Disorder: Results from the; National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R). 23. Kim et al. (2005). Physical health, depression and cognitive function as correlates of disability in an older Korean population. Correlational quantitative empirical study 18. Hudson et al. (2008). Clinical correlates of quality of life in systemic sclerosis measured with the World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule II. Canada Canada India South Korea Correlational quantitative 500 1204 South Korea Correlational quantitative empirical study 207 9090 Germany 0 402 337 40 Epidemiological United correlational States quantitative empirical study Psychometric quantitative empirical study Analytical study Switzerland Correlational quantitative empirical study 17. Goyal et al. (2002). Efficacy of Menosan, a polyherbal formulation in the management of menopausal syndrome with respect to quality of life. Psychiatry Psychiatry Medicine Medicine Psychiatry and medicine Medicine Medicine Medicine Survey on independent associations between physical health, depression, cognitive function and disability in the older Korean population. Survey on the role of a genotype (val66met) of the neurotrophic factor Survey on prevalence, correlation and clinical relevance of the DSM disorders and assessment of treatments adequacy. Convergent validity study of the German version of the Multidimensional Quality of Life Questionnaire for HIV/AIDS on a sample of HIV-infected patients. Study of characteristics of the WHODAS-S as a clinical tool for evaluation of individual functioning in psychiatric subjects. Study of validity of the WHODAS II in patients with systemic sclerosis. Identification of clinical features of systemic sclerosis that best correlate with the quality of life related to the health of patients. the WHODAS II. Assessment of the effects of Menosan, a polyherbal formulation, on quality of life in menopausal women. Correlation confirmed. Correlations confirmed. Detection of a good utility and ease of use and acceptable reliability for use by clinicians belonging to different schools and psychiatric traditions. Good validity and reliability of the Multidimensional Quality of Life Questionnaire for HIV/AIDS; convergent validity demonstrated. Prevalence, correlates and clinical relevance identified; inadequacy of treatment detected. Good validity. Clinical correlates identified. Correlation confirmed; efficacy of Menosan demonstrated. International Literature Rewiew on WHODAS II 96 380 Spain United States Qualitative empirical study of translation and adaptation Correlational quantitative empirical study 30. McKibbin et al. (2004). Assessing Disability in Older Patients With Schizophrenia Results From the WHODAS-II. 31. Mubarak AR. (2005). Social functioning and quality of life of people with schizophrenia in the northern region of Malaysia. 32. Norton et al. (2004). Psychiatric morbidity, disability and service use amongst primary care attenders in France. 33. Perini et al. (2006). Generic effectiveness measures: Sensitivity to symptom change in anxiety 130 New Zealand Correlational quantitative empirical study 124 Malaysia France Australia Correlational quantitative empirical study Correlational quantitative empirical study Correlational quantitative empirical study 169 258 United States Psychometric quantitative empirical study 76 70 845 27. MaGPIe Research Group. (2003). The nature and prevalence of psychological problems in New Zealand primary healthcare: a report on Mental Health and General Practice Investigation (MaGPIe). 28. Matías-Carrelo et al. (2003). The Spanish translation and cultural adaptation of five mental health outcome measures. 29. McArdle et al. (2005). The WHODAS II: measuring outcomes of hearing aid intervention for adults. New Zealand Correlational quantitative empirical study 26. MaGPIe Research Group. (2004). General practitioner recognition of mental illness in the absence of a ‘gold standard’. 86 Qualitative empirical study 25. Lastra et al. (2000). The classification of first episode schizophrenia: a cluster-analytical approach. Spain empirical study association between incident stroke and depression. Medicine Psychiatry Medicine Disability and rehabilitatio n Medicine Medicine Medicine Psychiatry Psychiatry Investigation on the relationship between social functioning and quality of life of people with schizophrenia in Malaysia. Investigation on the relationship between psychiatric morbidity, disability and use of services in French patients. Study with convergent measures on sensitivity to change in people with anxiety disorders. Assessment of reactivity of the WHODAS II to the short and long term effects in applications of acoustic devices. Evaluation of reliability and validity of the WHODAS II in older patients with schizophrenia. Spanish translation and adaptation of five measures of mental health. Study of the degree of disability and other factors that influence the recognition, management, course and outcome of mental disorders in patients of New Zealand. Comparison between the general practice of recognition of mental illness and the cases identified by diagnostic instruments and screening. derived from the brain (BDNF) in the association between stroke and depression. Check the classification of a schizophrenic population into subgroups for similar symptoms profiles. Convergent validity demonstrated. Correlations confirmed. Strong evidence of good reliability and some evidence of good validity. Correlation confirmed. Good reactivity of the WHODAS II, correlation detected. Semantic, technical and content equivalence demonstrated. Correlation is not verified; variability between instruments and between clinical opinion and screening and diagnostic tests. Correlations confirmed. Division into subgroups confirmed, but not predictive. Life Span and Disability Federici S. / Meloni F. / Lo Presti A. 168 Poland Epidemiological United correlational States quantitative empirical study Germany New Zealand Correlational quantitative empirical study Psychometric quantitative empirical study Correlational quantitative empirical study Correlational quantitative empirical study 9282 United States Correlational quantitative empirical study 42. Scott et al. (2008). Mentalphysical co-morbidity and its relationship with disability: results 1000 Netherlands Psychometric quantitative empirical study New Zealand 697 12992 134 276 904 36. Post et al. (2008). Development and validation of IMPAC T-S, an ICF-based questionnaire to measure activities and participation. 37. Pyne et al. (2003). Comparing the Sensitivity of Generic Effectiveness Measures With Symptom Improvement in Persons With Schizophrenia. 38. Pyszel et al. (2006). Disability, psychological distress and quality of life in breast cancer survivors with arm lymphedema. 39. Roth et al. (2006). Sleep Problems, Comorbid Mental Disorders, and Role Functioning in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. 40. Schlote et al. (2008). Use of the WHODAS II with stroke patients and their relatives: reliability and inter-rater-reliability. 41. Scott et al. (2006). Disability in Te Rau Hinengaro: The New Zealand Mental Health Survey. Germany Psychometric quantitative empirical study 35. Pösl et al. (2007). Psychometric properties of the WHODAS II in rehabilitation patients. 32 Quantitative and Sweden longitudinal empirical study disorders. 34. Pettersson et al. (2006). The effect of an outdoor powered wheelchair on activity and participation in users with stroke. Medicine Disability and rehabilitatio n Psychiatry Psychiatry Medicine Disability and rehabilitatio n Disability and rehabilitatio n Medicine Disability and rehabilitatio n Study on relationship between the disability and the presence of mental disorders and chronic physical conditions in the population of New Zealand, controlling comorbidity, age and sex. Survey on mental-physical comorbidity and on its relationship with disability. Assessment of disability, psychological distress and quality of life in breast cancer Polish survivors with arm lymphedema. National survey on the prevalence of sleep disorders, or the associations of sleep disorders with role disorders related to comorbidity of mental disorders. Measurement of the reliability of WHODAS II with stroke patients and their relatives. Study with convergent measures on the sensitivity of generic effectiveness in improving the symptoms of people with schizophrenia. Validation of the IMPAC T-S, an ICFbased questionnaire to measure activity and participation. Validation of the German version of the WHODAS II. Self-evaluation of the limitations in activities and restrictions in the participation of people with stroke, before and after the use of an outdoor powered wheelchair. Small correlation identified. Correlations identified. Good reliability. Correlations confirmed. Correlations confirmed. Good concurrent validity, test-retest reliability and internal consistency. Convergent validity demonstrated. Good validity and reliability and factorial structure confirmed. Positive effects of wheelchair found. International Literature Rewiew on WHODAS II 97 98 Spain United States United States Psychometric quantitative empirical study Correlational quantitative empirical study Psychometric quantitative empirical study Canada Netherlands Correlational quantitative empirical study Correlational quantitative empirical study Turkey Psychometric quantitative empirical study 45. Ulug et al. (2001). Reliability and validity of the Turkish version of the World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule-II (WHO-DAS-II) in schizophrenia. 46. van Tubergen et al. (2003). Assessment of disability with the World Health Organisation Disability Assessment Schedule II in patients with ankylosing spondylitis. 47. Vázquez-Barquero et al. (2000). Spanish version of the new World Health Organization Disablement Assessment Schedule II. 48. Von Korff et al. (2005). Potentially Modifiable Factors Associated With Disability Among People With Diabetes. 49. Von Korff et al. (2008). Modified WHODAS-II provides valid measure of global disability but filter items increased skewness. 50. Wang et al. (2006). Mental health and related disability among workers: A population-based study. Review of selfGermany administered measures on the health 44. Stucki et al. (2003). Assessment of the impact of disease on the individual. Norway Prospective quantitative empirical study 43. Soberg et al. (2007). Long-term multidimensional functional consequences of severe multiple injuries two years after trauma: a prospective longitudinal cohort study. from the World Mental Health Surveys. 5383 934 4357 163 214 90 0 105 Medicine Medicine Medicine Psychiatry Medicine Psychiatry Medicine Medicine Survey on the prevalence of psychiatric syndromes and related disability in a population of adult workers. Validation of a modified version of the WHODAS II with filter items. Identification of potentially modifiable factors associated with disability in people with diabetes. Validation of the Spanish version of the WHODAS II. Convergent validity study in patients with ankylosing spondylitis. Validation of the Turkish version of the WHODAS II in patients with schizophrenia. Implementation of an algorithm for the selection of current measures for the assessment of health conditions. Evaluation, through prospective cohort study, of the functioning and quality of life in patients with severe multiple injuries. Good reliability and general validity, but the use of filter questions adversely affects the properties of the instrument. Prevalence and correlations identified. Correlations identified; identification of factors. Good validity and reliability and factorial structure confirmed. Convergent validity demonstrated. About the WHODAS states that the validity and reliability of the instrument are still under investigation. Good reliability and validity. Correlation identified. Life Span and Disability Federici S. / Meloni F. / Lo Presti A. Korea Correlational quantitative and qualitative empirical study 1. Baumgartner J.N. (2004). Measuring disability and social integration among adults with psychotic disorders in Dar es Salaam, Tanzania. Tanzania Nationality Total of subjects Type of study Dissertations 88844 245 Subjects 267 200 Italy and United States Correlational quantitative empirical study Subtotal of subjects 67 Canada Psychometric quantitative empirical study Subjects 1. Baron et al. (2005). Preliminary study of the validity of the World Health Organization Disease Assessment Schedule (WHODAS II) in patients with scleroderma. 2. Federici et al. (2003). A CrossCultural Analysis of Relationships between Disability Self-Evaluation and Individual Predisposition to Use Assistive Technology. Nationality 88332 1204 Type of study Subtotal of subjects Psychometric quantitative empirical study Studies included in conferences 51. Yoon et al. (2004). Development of Korean version of World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule II (WHODAS II-K) in Community Dwelling Elders. Area of research Psychiatry Disability and rehabilitatio n Area of research Medicine Neuropsychi atry Study on the relationship between the severity of self-perceived disability and indicators of social integration (marital status, fertility and employment) in adult patients with psychotic disorders in Tanzania. Correlation confirmed between severity of selfperceived disability and one indicator of social integration: the employment. Results Correlations identified. Study on correlation between disability self-evaluation, individual coping strategies and individual predisposition to the use of assistive technologies. Purposes Good validity. Results Good validity and reliability and factorial structure confirmed. Study on psychometric characteristics of the WHODAS II in patients with scleroderma. Purposes Validation of the Korean version of the WHODAS II with elderly subjects. International Literature Rewiew on WHODAS II 99 Life Span and Disability Federici S. / Meloni F. / Lo Presti A. 3. Review of international literature on the WHODAS II Among the 54 studies identified by following the method described above, 51 are articles published in international journals, 2 were included in the conferences and one is a dissertation. However, only seven articles were published in journals or acts of conferences whose main object of interest is disability and rehabilitation (Federici, Scherer, Micangeli, Lombardo, & Olivetti Belardinelli, 2003; Annicchiarico, Gibert, Cortes, Campana, & Caltagirone, 2004; Gallagher & Mulvany, 2004; Chisolm, Abrams, McArdle, Wilson, & Doyle, 2005; McArdle, Chisolm, Abrams, Wilson, & Doyle, 2005; Pettersson, Törnquist, & Ahlström, 2006; Federici, Meloni, Mancini, Lauriola, & Olivetti Belardinelli, 2009). The remaining works were published in journals of medicine and psychiatry; the main purpose of these studies is the identification of correlations on comorbidity evaluations performed by clinicians about certain mental disorders. All these studies have investigated the correlation between the 6 domains of the WHODAS and/or its total score with the scores obtained on scales measuring depression (Alexopoulos, Raue, & Areán, 2003; Chwastiak & Von Korff, 2003; Kemmler et al., 2003; Kessler et al., 2003; McKibbin, Patterson, & Jeste, 2004; Yoon et al., 2004; Kim et al., 2005; Von Korff et al., 2005; Scott, McGee, Wells, & Browne, 2006; Banerjee et al., 2008), pain (Chwastiak & Von Korff, 2003; Stucki & Sigl, 2003; Pyszel, Malyszczak, Pyszel, Andrzejak, & Szuba, 2006; Soberg, Bautz-Holter, Roise, & Finset, 2007), schizophrenia and psychotic disorders (Janca et al., 1996; Lastra et al., 2000; Ulug, Ertugrul, Gögüs, & Kabakçi, 2001; Pyne, Sullivan, Kaplan, & Williams, 2003; Baumgartner, 2004; McKibbin et al., 2004; Norton, de Roquefeuil, Benjamins, Boulenger, & Mann, 2004; Mubarak, 2005; Chopra et al., 2008), quality of life (Goyal & Kulkarni, 2002; Kemmler et al., 2003; Pyne, Sullivan, Kaplan, & Williams, 2003; Chopra, Couper, & Herrman, 2004; ESEMeD/MHEDEA 2000 investigators, 2004; Donmez, Gokkoca, & Dedeoglu, 2005; Mubarak, 2005; Pyszel, Malyszczak, Pyszel, Andrzejak, & Szuba, 2006; Pösl, Miriam, Alarcos Cieza, & Gerold Stucki, 2007; Soberg, Bautz-Holter, Roise, & Finset, 2007; Baron et al., 2008; Hudson, Steele, Taillefer, & Baron, 2008; Hudson, Thombs, Steele, Watterson, Taillefer & Baron, 2008), sleep disorders (Roth et al., 2006), diabetes (Von Korff et al., 2005), ageing (Alexopoulos et al., 2003;Yoon et al., 2004; Kim et al., 2005; Donmez, Gokkoca & Dedeoglu, 2005), rheumatic disorders (Stucki & Sigl, 2003; van Tubergen et al., 2003; Baron, Hudson, & Taillefer, 2005), anxiety disorders (Bonnewyn, Bruffaerts, Van Oyen, Demarest, & Demyttenaere, 2005; Perini, Slade, & Andrews, 2006), strokes (Schlote et al., 2008), coping skills (Badr et al., 2007), cognitive functions (Kim et al., 2008), limitations of activity and restrictions in participation (Post et al., 2008) or in epidemiological and comorbidity national and international surveys (Kessler et al., 2003; MaGPIe Research Group, 2003; ESEMeD/MHEDEA 2000 investi100 International Literature Rewiew on WHODAS II gators, 2004; MaGPIe Research Group, 2004; Bonnewyn et al., 2005; Donmez et al., 2005; Wang, Adair, & Patten, 2006; Buist-Bouwman et al., 2008; Scott et al., 2008). The results obtained in these studies emphasize, first, that the WHODAS II is a useful, reliable and valid tool for assessment of disability, functioning and social participation, and is sensitive to changes like the SF-36 (Medical Outcomes Study Short Form 36); secondly, it facilitates the use of the ICF as a conceptual framework for the assessment of the limitations in activity and participation, and effectively discriminates between normal/healthy and disabled/sick people (Ertugrul & Ulug, 2004). Some studies suggest to using the WHODAS II together with the SF-36 (Chwastiak & Von Korff, 2003; Pyne et al., 2003; Baron et al., 2005; Von Korff et al., 2005; Perini et al., 2006; Soberg et al., 2007) or with the WHO Quality of Life – short version (WHQOL-BREF) in order to improve the health profile (Goyal & Kulkarni, 2002; Kemmler et al., 2003; Chopra et al., 2004) or together with Coping Inventory for Stressful Situations (CISS) and Matching Person and Technology (MPT) to assess the individual coping strategies and the predispositions to assistive technologies (Federici et al., 2003). Actually, the WHODAS II is a tool relatively complex and difficult to administer with full cooperation in psychiatric patients who reported that they were healthy and denied “emotional or mental problems” as described in the WHODAS II (Chopra et al., 2004, p. 757). Among the 51 articles, only eight have investigated the psychometric properties of the WHODAS II (Vázquez-Barquero et al., 2000; Ulug et al., 2001; Yoon et al., 2004; Baron et al., 2005; Chisolm et al., 2005; Buist-Bouwman et al., 2008; Von Korff et al., 2008; Federici et al., 2009) and one reports the translation into Spanish and its adaptation to the Latino culture (Matías-Carrelo et al., 2003). Vázquez-Barquero and his/her collaborators (Vázquez-Barquero et al., 2000) have studied the development of the Spanish version of the WHODAS II through a pilot cross-cultural analysis with 54 Spanish, 50 Cubans and 59 Peruvians, male and female, adults. Factor analysis, analysis of redundancy and missing values were conducted. The scores of the modified version of the instrument were compared with those of other countries. The Authors, however, failed to reach a clear and definitive assessment of the tool, merely to suggest further study on its psychometric properties. Ulug et al. (2001) have assessed the reliability and validity of Turkish version of the WHODAS II, in a study with 60 patients diagnosed with schizophrenia. The Cronbach’s Alpha, calculated for each of the six domains, reached values between .60 and .90, making possible to assess an acceptable internal consistency of the instrument. Regarding construct validity, domain scores displayed significant positive correlations with each other as well as with the total DAS score. According to the Authors, therefore, the WHODAS II is able to distinguish patients from control subjects; in ad101 Life Span and Disability Federici S. / Meloni F. / Lo Presti A. dition, the results show that the Turkish version of the instrument has satisfactory requirements of validity and reliability. The study of Yoon et al. (2004) was conducted to assess the Korean version of the WHODAS II, the sample consisted of 1204 elderly (aged 65 years or over) South Korean, community residents. In this study the WHODAS II-K showed high levels of internal consistency and reliability (splithalf, inter-rater and test-retest reliability). In the correlation analyses, scores on the WHODAS II-K were significantly correlated with the unfavorable conditions in all variables on health condition and contextual factors. Partial correlations of scores on the WHODAS II-K with the health condition were significant even after controlling for contextual factors. Therefore, the conclusion of the authors is that the WHODAS II-K is a reliable and valid instrument for assessing disability in elderly population. More recently, a preliminary study of validity was conducted on 67 Canadian subjects suffering from scleroderma. (The title of the poster appears as substantially confusing. We have attributed this to a misprint). The short abstract also does not provide sufficient information for an assessment of the study. Chisolm et al. (2005) examined the psychometric properties of the English version of the WHODAS II, in a sample of 380 adults with hearing loss. The results of the analysis of convergent validity showed that the WHODAS II-E is correlated with the scores of the Abbreviated Profile of Hearing Aid Benefit (APHAB), the Hearing Aid Handicap for the Elderly (HHIE), and the SF-36 (short form). The internal consistency of scores in different domains was satisfactory, except for the domain “Interactions and relationships with others”. The test-retest stability was adequate for the scores of all domains. Buist-Bouwman et al., (2008) have assessed the factorial structure, the internal consistency and the discriminant validity of the ESEMeD version of the WHODAS II, that is used in a European Study of Epidemiology of Mental Disorders.The sample was 8796 adults.The study confirms the structure of six factors of the WHODAS II, finds a good internal consistency of the instrument and also the results of discriminant validity appear, on a preliminary analysis, as acceptable. Finally, Von Korff et al. (2008) consider the psychometric properties of a WHODAS II modified for use in the World Mental Health Surveys with the addition of filter items in different subscales. Internal consistency and validity of the modified WHODAS II are generally supported, but the use of filter questions impairs measurement properties of the instrument. The most comprehensive psychometric analysis conducted, to date, on the WHODAS II is the work of Pösl et al. (2007), from a doctoral thesis, unpublished, of M. Pösl (2004), under the direction of G. Stucki, University of Monaco. The Authors evaluated the usefulness of the WHODAS II for measuring functioning and disability in patients with musculoskeletal diseases, internal diseases, stroke, breast cancer, and depressive disorder. The 102 International Literature Rewiew on WHODAS II validation of the German version of the WHODAS II was conducted in a sample of 904 patients from 19 rehabilitation centers and clinics in Bavaria.There was,among other things, a convergent validity with the SF-36.The conclusions of the study confirm the structure of six domains of the WHODAS II; furthermore, the instrument appears reliable and valid,and shows a sensitivity to change similar to that of the SF36 in the corresponding subscales. Given all the studies mentioned above, the WHODAS II was translated into the following languages: Italian (Federici et al., 2003;Annicchiarico et al., 2004; ESEMeD/MHEDEA 2000 investigators, 2004; Federici et al., 2009), English (Janca et al., 1996; Goyal & Kulkarni, 2002; Alexopoulos et al., 2003; Chwastiak & Von Korff, 2003; Kessler et al., 2003; MaGPIe Research Group, 2003; Pyne J.M., Sullivan et al., 2003; Baumgartner, 2004; Chopra et al., 2004; ESEMeD/MHEDEA 2000 investigators, 2004; Gallagher & Mulvany, 2004; McKibbin et al., 2004; MaGPIe Research Group, 2004; Baron et al., 2005; Chisolm et al., 2005; McArdle et al., 2005; Mubarak, 2005; Von Korff et al., 2005; Perini et al., 2006; Roth et al., 2006; Scott et al. 2006; Wang et al., 2006; Baron et al., 2008; Hudson et al., 2008), Swedish (Pettersson et al., 2006), Dutch (van Tubergen et al., 2003; ESEMeD/MHEDEA 2000 investigators, 2004;), German (Kemmler et al., 2003; Stucki & Sigl, 2003; ESEMeD/MHEDEA 2000 investigators, 2004; Pösl, 2007; Schlote et al., 2008), Korean (Yoon et al., 2004; Kim et al., 2005), Polish (Pyszel et al., 2006), Norwegian (Soberg et al., 2007), Turkish (Ulug et al., 2001; Ertugrul & Ulug, 2004; Donmez et al., 2005), Spanish (Lastra et al., 2000; Vázquez-Barquero et al., 2000; Matías-Carrelo et al., 2003; ESEMeD/MHEDEA 2000 investigators, 2004), French (Norton et al., 2004; ESEMeD/MHEDEA 2000 investigators, 2004; Bonnewyn et al., 2005), Arabic (Badr et al., 2007). Korean, Polish and Swedish translations are not provided by WHO (WHO, 2004). In conclusion, the review of international literature on the WHODAS shows a broad consensus on the reliability and validity of the instrument, although the lack of standardized scores for the different translations of the WHODAS and the scarcity of particularly thorough studies does not guarantee that the cultural and psychometric requirements have been met by the instrument. 4. Characteristics of the Italian version of the WHODAS II The study of Federici et al. had as general aim to provide a contribution to the validation of the Italian version of the WHODAS II, considering the widespread consent about the usefulness of the tool. Specifically, the Authors wanted to test if the WHODAS II can be regarded as a reliable instrument to assess the functioning and the self-perception of disability in persons with normal abilities and disabled participants, by the means of the analysis of some psychometric characteristics such as the reliability (inter103 Life Span and Disability Federici S. / Meloni F. / Lo Presti A. nal homogeneity, Cronbach’s Alpha) and the validity (principal components analysis). The Italian version of the WHODAS II has been adapted by the Authors in the same format as the English one (36-Item Interviewer Administered, Day Codes Version – February 2000), because this was the most recent version of the instrument. The Authors have deleted the Italian items of the sections 3 and 5, since they were not further included in the last format of the English version. The WHODAS II was administered to a sample of 500 participants (185 males and 315 females,) divided into two sub-samples: 271 normal adults and 229 disabled adults. Moreover, the disabled participant group comprised 111 motor disabled, 45 mental disabled and 73 sensory disabled. The findings obtained show a good correspondence with the original structure of the WHODAS II. Furthermore, the internal consistency of most subscales, estimated by means of the Cronbach’s Alpha, was found to be high in the examined sample. Regarding the factorial structure of the instrument, the results confirm the presence of six main factors, according to the six activity domains expected to be assessed by the WHODAS II. The study of Federici et al. presents, however, some limitations: first, the three subgroups of disabled do not match each other for participant number, age and sex; moreover, the enrolment of mental disabled respondents ran into difficulties because it was not easy to access the centres for mental disabled in Italy. Finally, neither the convergent validity nor the reliability test – re-test of the instrument- has been studied. A research prosecution is therefore desirable which proposes, among other things, achieving standard scores for each macro-category of disability. Normative scores of disability would be useful to integrate the self-evaluation of a single individual regarding his/her functioning in a specific context. Indeed, by comparing the disability self-evaluation of a single individual to standard scores it will be possible to assess how much each factor of the biopsychosocial determinants of the individual’s functioning influences the disability self-evaluation of that person. 5. Conclusions The WHODAS II is a tool for the self-evaluation of limitations in activities and restrictions in participation experienced by an individual, independently from a medical diagnosis. The self- evaluation of the instrument appears a fundamental element compared to tests or questionnaires traditionally used for the assessment of disability, which usually reveal the point of view of the caregiver or clinician who compiles them. The revolution in the conception of disability, functioning and health represented by the biopsycho-social model and the new International Classification (ICF), con104 International Literature Rewiew on WHODAS II ceptually compatible with it, reveals the absolute priority of an individual subjective perspective, compared to any other etiopathological assessment, both the objective and reducing-individual-to-object point of view of the clinician. The increasingly widespread utilization of the bio-psycho-social model at international level and the simultaneous promotion of the use of the new classification, have brought, in recent years, even increasing use of the new assessment tools, above all WHODAS II. This has involved, first, the need to accurately analyze the psychometric properties of the instrument, and in particular its reliability, stability, internal consistency, convergent validity and factorial structure. This study has reviewed all studies published (until 2008) in the major scientific search engines, where has described the use and/or validation of WHODAS II. Research conducted identified 54 studies: 51 articles in international journals, 2 included in conferences and a doctoral dissertation. Of these, only six articles were published in journals or acts of conferences whose main object of interest is disability and rehabilitation. All studies considered have assessed the degree of correlation between the scores of the WHODAS II and the scores obtained by subjects on rating scales related to: depression, pain, schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders, quality of life, sleep disorders, diabetes, ageing , rheumatic disorders, anxiety disorders. All studies reviewed agree that the WHODAS II is an useful instrument for the assessment of disability, functioning and social participation, suggesting quite often to join the administration with scales used for measuring quality of life (eg.: SF-36 or WHQOL-BREF). Among the 51 articles only eight, however, have investigated the psychometric properties of the instrument, concluding, almost unanimously, that the psychometric properties of the WHODAS II allow it to be to considered a valid and reliable instrument for the assessment of disability. Among the main limitations that this review has helped to highlight, it is important to note that, to date, there are no standardized scores for the various translations of the WHODAS and that the number of studies that sought to investigate in detail the psychometric properties of the tool is particularly limited. Therefore, it would be desirable not only to universalize the tool, but also to deepen the studies conducted so far, in order to determine more precisely the advantages and limitations of WHODAS II. 105 Life Span and Disability Federici S. / Meloni F. / Lo Presti A. References Alexopoulos, G. S., Raue, P., & Areán, P. (2003). Problem-solving therapy versus supportive therapy in geriatric major depression with executive dysfunction. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 11(1), 46-52. Annicchiarico, R. O., Gibert, K., Cortes, U., Campana, F., & Caltagirone, C. (2004). Qualitative profiles of disability. Journal of Rehabilitation Research & Development, 41(6), 835-846. Badr, H. E., & Abd El Aziz, H. M. (2007). Role of Gender in Coping Capabilities among Young Visually Disabled Students. The Journal of the Egyptian Public Health Association, 82 (5-6), 365-77. Banerjee, A., Kumar, S., Kulhara, P., & Gupta, A. (2008). Prevalence of depression and its effect on disability in patients with age-related macular degeneration. Indian Journal of Ophthalmology, 56 (6), 469-74. Baron, M., Hudson, M., & Taillefer, S. (2005). Preliminary study of the validity of the World Health Organization Disease [sic.] Assessment Schedule (WHODAS II) in patients with scleroderma (ssc). Annals of the Rheumatic Diseases, 64 (Suppl 3), 948. Baron, M., Schieir, O., Hudson, M., Steele, R., Kolahi, S., Berkson, L., Couture, F., Fitzcharles, M. A., Gagné, M., Garfield, B., Gutkowski, A., Kang, H., Kapusta, M., Ligier, S., Mathieu, J. P., Ménard, H., Starr, M., Stein, M., & Zummer, M. (2008). The clinimetric properties of the World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule II in early inflammatory arthritis. Arthritis & Rheumatism, 59 (3), 382-390. Baumgartner, J. N. (2004). Measuring disability and social integration among adults with psychotic disorders in Dar es Salaam, Tanzania. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 65 (4-B). Bickenbach, J. E., Chatterji, S., Badley, E. M., & Üstün, T. B. (1999). Models of disablement, universalism and the international classification of impairments, disabilities and handicaps. Social Science and Medicine, 48 (9), 1173-1187. Bonnewyn, A., Bruffaerts, R., Van Oyen, H., Demarest, S., & Demyttenaere, K. (2005). The impact of mental disorders on daily functioning in the Belgian community. Results of the study “European Study on Epidemiology of Mental Disorders” (ESemeD). Revue Medicale De Liege, 60 (11), 849-54. Buist-Bouwman, M., Ormel, J., De Graaf, R., Vilagut, G., Alonso, J., Van Sonderen, E., Vollebergh, W., & The ESEMeD/MHEDEA 2000 Investigators. (2008). Psychometric properties of the World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule used in the European Study of the Epidemiology of Mental Disorders. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 17 (4), 185-197. Buono, S., & Zagaria, T. (1999). “From disability to activity, from handicap to par106 International Literature Rewiew on WHODAS II ticipation”: The new guidelines in the classifications of the World Health Organization. Life Span and Disability, 2 (1), 93-113. Bury, M. (2000).A comment on the ICIDH2. Disability & Society, 15 (7), 1073-1077. Chamie, M. (1995). What does morbidity have to do with disability? Disability and Rehabilitation, 17 (7), 323-337. Chatterji, S., Üstün, T. B., & Trotter, R. T. (2001). Objectives and Overall Plan for the ICIDH-2 Cross-Cultural Applicability Study. In T. B. Üstün et al. Disability and Culture: universalism and diversity (pp. 21-36). Seattle: Hogrefe & Huber. Chisolm, T. H., Abrams, H. B., McArdle, R., Wilson, R. H., & Doyle, P. J. (2005). The WHO-DAS II: psychometric properties in the measurement of functional health status in adults with acquired hearing loss. Trends in Amplification, 9 (3), 111-126. Chopra, P. K., Couper, J. W., & Herrman, H. (2004). The assessment of patients with long-term psychotic disorders: Application of the WHO Disability Assessment Schedule II. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 38 (9), 753-759. Chopra, P. K., Herrman, H., & Kennedy, G. (2008). Comparison of disability and quality of life measures in patients with long-term psychotic disorders and patients with multiple sclerosis: an application of the WHO Disability Assessment Schedule II and WHO Quality of Life-BREF. International Journal of Rehabilitation Research, 31 (2), 141-9. Chwastiak, L. A., & Von Korff, M. (2003). Disability in depression and back pain: evaluation of the World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule (WHO DAS II) in a primary care setting. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 56 (6), 507-14. Donmez, L., Gokkoca, Z., & Dedeoglu, N. (2005). Disability and its effects on quality of life among older people living in Antalya city center, Turkey. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 40 (2), 213-223. Ertugrul, A., & Ulug, B. (2004). Perception of stigma among patients with schizophrenia. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 39 (1), 73-77. ESEMeD/MHEDEA 2000 investigators. (2004). Disability and quality of life impact of mental disorders in Europe: results from the European Study of the Epidemiology of Mental Disorders (ESEMeD) project. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 109 (Suppl. 420), 38-46. Federici, S., Meloni, F., Mancini, A., Lauriola, M., & Olivetti Belardinelli, M. (2009). World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule II (WHODAS II): A contribution to the Italian validation. Disability and Rehabilitation. Federici, S., Scherer, M. J., Micangeli, A., Lombardo, C., & Olivetti Belardinelli, M. (2003). A Cross-Cultural Analysis of Relationships between Disability Self-Evalu- 107 Life Span and Disability Federici S. / Meloni F. / Lo Presti A. ation and Individual Predisposition to Use Assistive Technology. In G. M. Craddock, L. P. McCormack, R. B. Reilly, & H. T. P. Knops (Eds.), Assistive Technology Shaping the Future (pp. 941-946). Amsterdam: IOS Press. Gallagher, P., & Mulvany, F. (2004). Levels of ability and functioning: using the WHODAS II in an Irish context. Disability and Rehabilitation, 26 (9), 506-17. Goyal, U., & Kulkarni, K. S. (2002). Efficacy of Menosan, a polyherbal formulation in the management of menopausal syndrome with respect to quality of life. Indian Journal of Clinical Practice, 13(8), 37-40. Hudson,M.,Steele.R.,Taillefer,S.,Baron,M.,& Canadian Scleroderma Research.(2008). Quality of life in systemic sclerosis: psychometric properties of the World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule II. Arthritis and Rheumatism, 59 (2), 270-278. Hudson, M., Thombs, B. D., Steele, R., Watterson, R., Taillefer, S., Baron, M., & Canadian Scleroderma Research Group Investigators of the Canadian Scleroderma Research Group. (2008). Clinical correlates of quality of life in systemic sclerosis measured with the World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule II. Arthritis and Rheumatism, 59 (2), 279-284. Janca,A., Kastrup, M., Katschnig, H., Lopez-Ibor, J. J. Jr., Mezzich, J. E., & Sartorius, N. (1996). The World Health Organization Short Disability Assessment Schedule (WHO DAS-S): a tool for the assessment of difficulties in selected areas of functioning of patients with mental disorders. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 31 (6), 349-354. Keith, R. A., Granger, C. V., Hamilton, B. B., & Sherwin, F. S. (1987). The functional independence measure: a new tool for rehabilitation. Advances in Clinical Rehabilitation, 1, 6-18. Kemmler, G., Schmied, B., Shetty-Lee, A., Zangerle, R., Hinterhuber, H., G. Schüssler, & Mumelter, B. (2003). Quality of life of HIV-infected patients: Psychometric properties and validation of the German version of the MQOL-HIV. Quality of Life Research, 12 (8), 1037-1050. Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Koretz, D., Merikangas, K. R., Rush, A. J., Walters, E. E., Wang, P. S. (2003). The Epidemiology of Major Depressive Disorder: Results from the; National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R). Journal of the American Medical Association, 289 (23), 3095-3105. Kim, J. M., Stewart, R., Glozier, N., Prince, M., Kim, S. W., Yang, S. J., Shin, I. S., & Yoon, J. S. (2005). Physical health, depression and cognitive function as correlates of disability in an older Korean population. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 20 (2), 160-7. Kim, J. M., Stewart, R., Kim, S. W., Yang, S. J., Shin, I. S., Kim, Y. H., & Yoon, J. S. (2008). BDNF genotype potentially modifying the association between incident stroke and depression. Neurobiology of Aging, 29 (5), 789-792. 108 International Literature Rewiew on WHODAS II Lastra, I., Vãzquez-Barquero, J. L., Herrera Castanedo, S., Cuesta, M. J., VãzquezBourgon, M. E., & Dunn, G. (2000). The classification of first episode schizophrenia: a cluster-analytical approach. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 102 (1), 26-31. MaGPIe Research Group. (2003). The nature and prevalence of psychological problems in New Zealand primary healthcare: a report on Mental Health and General Practice Investigation (MaGPIe). The New Zealand Medical Journal, 116 (1171), U379. MaGPIe Research Group. (2004). General practitioner recognition of mental illness in the absence of a ‘gold standard’. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 38 (10), 789-794. Mahoney, F. I., & Barthel, D. W. (1965). Functional Evaluation: The Barthel Index. Maryland State Medical Journal, 14, 61-65. Matías-Carrelo, L. E., Chávez, L. M., Negrón, G., Canino, G., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., & Hoppe, S. (2003). The spanish translation and cultural adaptation of five mental health outcome measures. Culture, Medicine & Psychiatry, 27 (3), 291-313. McArdle, R., Chisolm, T. H., Abrams, H. B., Wilson, R. H., & Doyle, P. J. (2005). The WHO-DAS II: measuring outcomes of hearing aid intervention for adults. Trends in Amplification, 9 (3), 127-43. McKibbin, C., Patterson, T. L., & Jeste, D. V. (2004). Assessing Disability in Older Patients With Schizophrenia Results From the WHODAS-II. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 192 (6), 405-413. Mubarak, A. R. (2005). Social functioning and quality of life of people with schizophrenia in the northern region of Malaysia. Australian E-Journal for the Advancement of Mental Health, 4 (3), 1-10. Norton, J., de Roquefeuil, G., Benjamins, A., Boulenger, J. P., & Mann, A. (2004). Psychiatric morbidity, disability and service use amongst primary care attenders in France. European Psychiatry, 19 (3), 164-167. Perini, S. J., Slade, T., & Andrews, G. (2006). Generic effectiveness measures: Sensitivity to symptom change in anxiety disorders. Journal of Affective Disorders, 90 (23), 123-130. Pettersson, I., Törnquist, K., & Ahlström, G. (2006). The effect of an outdoor powered wheelchair on activity and participation in users with stroke. Disability and Rehabilitation: Assistive Technology, 1 (4), 235-243. Pfeiffer, D. (2000). The Devils are in the Details: the ICIDH2 and the disability movement. Disability & Society, 15 (7), 1079-1082. Pösl, M. (2004). Evaluation of the World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule II (WHO DAS II) - German Version. Disability in Patients with Muscu- 109 Life Span and Disability Federici S. / Meloni F. / Lo Presti A. loskeletal Diseases, Cardiovascular and General Internal Diseases, Stroke, Breast Cancer and Depressive Disorder. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Universität München, München, Germany. Available: http://deposit.ddb.de/cgi-bin/dokserv? idn=972295240&dok_var=d1&dok_ext=pdf&filename=972295240.pdf via the INTERNET. Accessed 2008 December 31. Pösl, M., Cieza, A., & Stucki, G. (2007). Psychometric properties of the WHODASII in rehabilitation patients. Quality of Life Research, 16 (9), 1521-1531. Post, M. W. M., de Witte, L. P., Reichrath, E., Verdonschot, M. M., Wijlhuizen, G. J., & Perenboom, R. J. M. (2008). Development and validation of IMPACT-S, an ICFbased questionnaire to measure activities and participation. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, 40 (8), 620-627. Pyne, J. M., Sullivan, G., Kaplan, R., & Williams, D. K. (2003). Comparing the Sensitivity of Generic Effectiveness Measures With Symptom Improvement in Persons With Schizophrenia. Medical Care, 41 (2), 208-217. Pyszel, A., Malyszczak, K., Pyszel, K., Andrzejak, R., & Szuba, A. (2006). Disability, psychological distress and quality of life in breast cancer survivors with arm lymphedema. Lymphology, 39 (4), 185-192. Rehm, J., Trotter, R. T., Chatterji, S., & Üstün, T. B. (2001). The Structure and Stability of the Proposed International Classification. In T. B. Üstün et al. Disability and Culture: universalism and diversity. Seattle: Hogrefe & Huber. Roth, T., Jaeger, S., Jin, R., Kalsekar, A., Stang, P. E., & Kessler, R. C. (2006). Sleep Problems, Comorbid Mental Disorders, and Role Functioning in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Biological Psychiatry, 60 (12), 1364-1371. Schlote, A., Richter, M., Wunderlich, M. T., Poppendick, U., Möller, C., & Wallesch, C. W. (2008). Use of the WHODAS II with Stroke Patients and Their Relatives: Reliability and Inter-Rater-Reliability. Die Rehabilitation, 47(1), 31-38. Scott, K. M., McGee, M. A., Wells, J. E., Browne, M. A. O., & New Zealand Mental Health Survey Research Team. (2006). Disability in Te Rau Hinengaro: The New Zealand Mental Health Survey. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 40 (10), 889-895. Scott, K. M., Von Korff, M., Alonso, J., Angermeyer, M., Bromet, E., Fayyad, J., de Girolamo, G., Demyttenaere, K., Gasquet, I., Gureje, O., Haro, J., He,Y., Kessler, R., Levinson, D., Medina Mora, M., Oakley Browne, M., Ormel, J., Posada-Villa, J., Watanabe, M., & Williams, D. (2008, Epub ahead of print). Mental-physical co-morbidity and its relationship with disability: results from the World Mental Health Surveys. Psychological Medicine. Soberg, H. L., Bautz-Holter, E., Roise, O., & Finset, A. (2007). Long-term multidimensional functional consequences of severe multiple injuries two years after trauma: a prospective longitudinal cohort study. The Journal of Trauma, 62 (2), 461-70. 110 International Literature Rewiew on WHODAS II Stucki, G., & Sigl, T. (2003). Assessment of the impact of disease on the individual. Best Practice & Research: Clinical Rheumatology, 17 (3), 451-473. Ulug, B., Ertugrul, A., Gögüs, A., & Kabakçi, E. (2001). Yetiyitimi Degerlendirme Çizelgesinin (WHO-DAS-II) sizofreni hastalarinda geçerlilik ve güvenilirligi. (Reliability and validity of the Turkish version of the World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule-II (WHO-DAS-II) in schizophrenia). Turk Psikiyatri Dergisi, 12 (2), 121-130. Üstün, T. B., Bickenbach, J. E., Badley, E., & Chatterji, S. (1998). “The ICIDH and the need for its revision”: Comment. Disability & Society, 13 (5), 829-831. Üstün, T. B., Chatterji, S., Bickenbach, J. E., Trotter, R. T., & Saxena, S. (2001). Disability and Cultural Variation:The ICIDH-2 Cross-Cultural Applicability Research Study. In T. B. Üstün et al. Disability and Culture: universalism and diversity (pp. 320). Seattle: Hogrefe & Huber. van Tubergen, A., Landewe, R., Heuft-Dorenbosch, L., Spoorenberg, A., van der Heijde, D., van der Tempel, H., & van der Linden, S. (2003). Assessment of disability with the World Health Organisation Disability Assessment Schedule II in patients with ankylosing spondylitis. Annals of the Rheumatic Diseases, 62 (2), 140-145. Vázquez-Barquero, J. L., Vázquez Bourgón, M. E., Herrera Castanedo, S., Saiz, J., Uriarte, M., Morales, F., Gaite, L., Herrán, A., Üstün, T. B., & Grupo Cantabria en Discapacidades. (2000). Versión en lengua española de un nuevo cuestionario de evaluación de discapacidades de la OMS (WHO-DAS-II): Fase initial de desarrollo y estudio piloto. / Spanish version of the new World Health Organization Disablement Assessment Schedule II (WHO-DAS-II): Initial phase of development and pilot study. Actas Españolas De Psiquiatría, 28 (2), 77-87. Von Korff, M., Crane, P., Alonso, J., Vilagut, G., Angermeyer, M., Bruffaerts, R., de Girolamo, G., Gureje, O., de Graaf, R., Huang, Y., Iwata, N., Karam, E., Kovess, V., Lara, C., Levinson, D., Posada-Villa, J., Scott, K., & Ormel, J. (2008, Epub ahead of print). Modified WHODAS-II provides valid measure of global disability but filter items increased skewness. Journal Clinical Epidemiology. Von Korff, M., Katon, W., Lin, E. H. B., Simon, G., Ludman, E., Oliver, M., Ciechanowski, P., Rutter, C., & Bush, T. (2005). Potentially Modifiable Factors Associated With Disability Among People With Diabetes. Psychosomatic Medicine, 67 (2), 233-240. Wang, J., Adair, C. E., & Patten, S. B. (2006). Mental health and related disability among workers: A population-based study. American Journal of Industrial Medicine, 49 (7), 514-522. World Health Organization. (WHO). (1948). Constitution of the World Health Organization, 22 July 1946 (RS 0.810.1). Geneva: WHO. 111 Life Span and Disability Federici S. / Meloni F. / Lo Presti A. World Health Organization (WHO). (1980). International Classification of Impairments, Disabilities, and Handicaps. A Manual of classification relating to the consequences of disease. Geneva: WHO. World Health Organization (WHO). (2001). International Classification of Functioning, Disability, and Health: ICF. Geneva: WHO. Yoon, J. S., Kim, J. M., Shin, I. S., Yang, S. J., Zheng, T. J., & Lee, H. Y. (2004). Development of Korean version of World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule II (WHODAS II-K) in Community Dwelling Elders. Journal of the Korean Neuropsychiatric Association, 43 (1), 86-92. 112